2020
DOI: 10.3758/s13423-020-01719-6
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Sequential sampling models without random between-trial variability: the racing diffusion model of speeded decision making

Abstract: Most current sequential sampling models have random between-trial variability in their parameters. These sources of variability make the models more complex in order to fit response time data, do not provide any further explanation to how the data were generated, and have recently been criticised for allowing infinite flexibility in the models. To explore and test the need of between-trial variability parameters we develop a simple sequential sampling model of N-choice speeded decision making: the racing diffu… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1

Citation Types

2
71
0

Year Published

2020
2020
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
4
3
1

Relationship

1
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 74 publications
(84 citation statements)
references
References 81 publications
2
71
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Even considering only model-free (as opposed to model-based (Daw and Dayan, 2014)) reinforcement learning, there exists a variety of learning rules (e.g., Palminteri et al, 2015; Rescorla and Wagner, 1972; Rummery and Niranjan, 1994; Sutton, Richard, 1988), as well as the possibility of multiple learning rates for positive and negative prediction errors (Christakou et al, 2013; Daw et al, 2002; Frank et al, 2009; Gershman, 2015; Haughey et al, 2007; Niv et al, 2012), and many additional concepts, such as eligibility traces to allow for updating of previously visited states (Barto et al, 1981; Bogacz et al, 2007). Similarly, in the decision-making literature, there exists a wide range of evidence-accumulation models, including most prominently the diffusion decision model (DDM; Ratcliff, 1978; Ratcliff et al, 2016) and race models such as the linear ballistic accumulator model (LBA; Brown and Heathcote, 2008) and racing diffusion (RD) models (Boucher et al, 2007; Hawkins and Heathcote, 2020; Leite and Ratcliff, 2010; Logan et al, 2014; Purcell et al, 2010; Ratcliff et al, 2011; Tillman et al, 2020).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Even considering only model-free (as opposed to model-based (Daw and Dayan, 2014)) reinforcement learning, there exists a variety of learning rules (e.g., Palminteri et al, 2015; Rescorla and Wagner, 1972; Rummery and Niranjan, 1994; Sutton, Richard, 1988), as well as the possibility of multiple learning rates for positive and negative prediction errors (Christakou et al, 2013; Daw et al, 2002; Frank et al, 2009; Gershman, 2015; Haughey et al, 2007; Niv et al, 2012), and many additional concepts, such as eligibility traces to allow for updating of previously visited states (Barto et al, 1981; Bogacz et al, 2007). Similarly, in the decision-making literature, there exists a wide range of evidence-accumulation models, including most prominently the diffusion decision model (DDM; Ratcliff, 1978; Ratcliff et al, 2016) and race models such as the linear ballistic accumulator model (LBA; Brown and Heathcote, 2008) and racing diffusion (RD) models (Boucher et al, 2007; Hawkins and Heathcote, 2020; Leite and Ratcliff, 2010; Logan et al, 2014; Purcell et al, 2010; Ratcliff et al, 2011; Tillman et al, 2020).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Recently, Osth and Farrell (2019) jointly modeled serial position and complete RT distributions with two different evidence accumulation models: the LBA and racing diffusion model (Tillman, Van Zandt, & Logan, 2020) to provide novel insights into primacy and recency effects in free recall initiation. Not only did this work account for both response probabilities and latency distributions, the modeling led to several novel insights.…”
Section: Evidence Accumulation Models Of the Free Recall Taskmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Response time (RT) distributions are right skewed despite many researchers treating the data as normal. The relative speed of correct and error responses are not equal (Laming, 1968;Ratcliff, 1978;Ratcliff & Rouder, 1998;Tillman, Van Zandt, & Logan, 2020), where the error RT is either faster than the correct RT on average (i.e., fast errors) or slower than the correct RT on average (i.e., slow errors). Slow errors occur when the choice is difficult or accuracy is emphasized and fast errors occur when the choice is easy or there is pressure to decide quickly (Swensson, 1972).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The second component is between-trial starting point variability, which allows the model to predict fast errors (Laming, 1968;Smith & Vickers, 1988;Ratcliff & Rouder, 1998;Ratcliff, Van Zandt, & McKoon, 1999). Tillman et al (2020) argued that there are several issues that arise when adding in additional sources of variability. First, sequential sampling models have become increasingly complex in order to fit RT data and adding in the variability parameters involves two numerical integrations that can be computationally demanding.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation