2020
DOI: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2019.116387
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Sequence learning in the human brain: A functional neuroanatomical meta-analysis of serial reaction time studies

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
3
1

Citation Types

12
70
1

Year Published

2020
2020
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
7
1
1

Relationship

1
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 106 publications
(106 citation statements)
references
References 82 publications
12
70
1
Order By: Relevance
“…Notwithstanding that the present study supports the dPMC's involvement in motor sequence learning, this notion has been challenged by a recent meta-analysis suggesting that particularly the basal ganglia directly contribute to motor sequence learning while premotor regions as well as the cerebellum do not [84]. Thus, it has been argued that increased activity in cerebellar and premotor regions during motor learning might reflect behavioral changes associated with motor learning rather than learning per se [28].…”
Section: Sequence Reproductioncontrasting
confidence: 67%
“…Notwithstanding that the present study supports the dPMC's involvement in motor sequence learning, this notion has been challenged by a recent meta-analysis suggesting that particularly the basal ganglia directly contribute to motor sequence learning while premotor regions as well as the cerebellum do not [84]. Thus, it has been argued that increased activity in cerebellar and premotor regions during motor learning might reflect behavioral changes associated with motor learning rather than learning per se [28].…”
Section: Sequence Reproductioncontrasting
confidence: 67%
“…Learning linked to the neurocircuitry that involves the BG, particularly the striatum, seems to encompass associative learning processes whereby we extract predictable regularities (e.g., repeating patterns) from the environment automatically, that is, without intention or conscious monitoring (Janacsek et al, 2020). This form of learning appears to be primarily external/stimulus-driven and occurs implicitly, without any awareness or cognitive effort (Janacsek et al, 2012).…”
Section: Competitive Neurocognitive Functions: An Integrative Accountmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…One aspect of cognitive function that can be adversely impacted by aging is procedural learning, which is a type of learning that occurs without an individual's intention and is often implicit (i.e., outside of awareness) (Zwart, Vissers, Kessels, & Maes, 2017). Implicit learning is essential to healthy functioning with the advancement of age in a plethora of everyday behaviours that require appropriate sequences, including typing, arithmetic operations, social interactions, reading and motor skills, and so forth (Janacsek et al, 2019).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…One aspect of cognitive function that can be adversely impacted by aging is procedural learning, which is a type of learning that occurs without an individual's intention and is often implicit (i.e., outside of awareness) (Zwart, Vissers, Kessels, & Maes, 2017). Implicit learning is essential to healthy functioning with the advancement of age in a plethora of everyday behaviours that require appropriate sequences, including typing, arithmetic operations, social interactions, reading and motor skills, and so forth (Janacsek et al, 2019). Procedural learning shows age‐related decline (Nemeth & Janacsek, 2011; Nemeth et al, 2011, 2013; Zwart et al, 2017), which may reflect (a) cognitive slowing in older adults from having multiple representations simultaneously activated, (b) associative binding deficits between multiple stimuli or stimulus features and binding these associations into long‐term memory traces; and (c) increased sensitivity to interference (Nemeth & Janacsek, 2011).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%