2016
DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.01943
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Semantically Transparent and Opaque Compounds in German Noun-Phrase Production: Evidence for Morphemes in Speaking

Abstract: This study examines the lexical representation and processing of noun-noun compounds and their grammatical gender during speech production in German, a language that codes for grammatical gender (masculine, feminine, and neuter). Using a picture-word interference paradigm, participants produced determiner-compound noun phrases in response to pictures, while ignoring written distractor words. Compound targets were either semantically transparent (e.g., birdhouse) or opaque (e.g., hotdog), and their constituent … Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

0
15
1
1

Year Published

2018
2018
2022
2022

Publication Types

Select...
6
1

Relationship

3
4

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 13 publications
(18 citation statements)
references
References 81 publications
0
15
1
1
Order By: Relevance
“…The absence of corresponding effects with familiar compounds, then, is in line with the assumption of a single-lemma representation view and does not provide support for the multiple-lemma representation view. Note, however, that our findings do not question the notion of morpheme-based storage of compounds at the word-form level (e.g., Lorenz & Zwitserlood, 2016; Lüttmann et al, 2011; Roelofs, 1996a, 1996b).…”
Section: Discussioncontrasting
confidence: 80%
See 3 more Smart Citations
“…The absence of corresponding effects with familiar compounds, then, is in line with the assumption of a single-lemma representation view and does not provide support for the multiple-lemma representation view. Note, however, that our findings do not question the notion of morpheme-based storage of compounds at the word-form level (e.g., Lorenz & Zwitserlood, 2016; Lüttmann et al, 2011; Roelofs, 1996a, 1996b).…”
Section: Discussioncontrasting
confidence: 80%
“…Naming latencies were faster in the related condition. In a subsequent study, similar effects were obtained when single constituents of compound target words were presented as distractors (e.g., bag-handbag, Lorenz & Zwitserlood, 2016). These effects cannot be attributed to semantic overlap because similar-sized effects were obtained for semantically transparent and opaque targets; for the latter, there is no overlap in meaning between compound and constituent (e.g., Zahn [tooth] -Löwenzahn ["lion tooth" = dandelion], for related evidence from studies with compound distractors and simple-noun targets, see Dohmes, Zwitserlood, & Bölte, 2004;Koester & Schiller, 2008;Lensink, Verdonschot, & Schiller, 2015).…”
Section: The Production Of Compoundsmentioning
confidence: 55%
See 2 more Smart Citations
“…Importantly, this was more pronounced in older speakers, pointing to deficient or delayed morpho-phonological encoding in the elderly. In models of speech production, interference effects of distractors from the same semantic category as the target (e.g., tulip → sunflower ) are taken to reflect lexical competition at the so-called “lemma” level, whereas morphological facilitation reflects activation of morpheme representations at the word-form level (Badecker, 2001; Levelt, Roelofs, & Meyer, 1999 see also Koester & Schiller, 2008; Lensink, Verdonschot, & Schiller, 2014; Lorenz & Zwitserlood, 2016; Lüttmann, Bölte, Böhl, & Zwitserlood, 2011 but see Janssen, Bi, & Caramazza, 2008; Janssen, Pajitas, & Caramazza, 2014). While age-related effects were restricted to the morphological distractor conditions, no ageing effects were obtained with semantic distractors.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%