2016
DOI: 10.1002/hbm.23304
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Semantic interference and its control: A functional neuroimaging and connectivity study

Abstract: During picture naming, the ease with which humans generate words is dependent upon the context in which they are named. For instances, naming previously presented items results in facilitation. Instead, naming a picture semantically related to previous items displays persistent interference effects (i.e., cumulative semantic interference, CSI). The neural correlates of CSI are still unclear and it is a matter of debate whether semantic control, or cognitive control more in general, is necessary for the resolut… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1

Citation Types

0
35
2
1

Year Published

2017
2017
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
7
1

Relationship

0
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 42 publications
(39 citation statements)
references
References 112 publications
0
35
2
1
Order By: Relevance
“…Blocked-cyclic and continuous naming tasks are used often interchangeably to draw inferences about the nature of semantic interference effects in naming and the processes that underlie speech production in general (Blocked Cyclic Naming: e.g., Belke, 2008, 2013; Damian & Als, 2005; de Zubicaray et al, 2014; Meinzer et al, 2016; Navarrete, Mahon, Lorenzoni, & Peressotti, 2016; Schnur et al, 2006, 2009; Continuous Naming: e.g., Belke, 2013; Canini et al, 2016; Howard et al, 2006; Navarrete et al, 2010; Ries et al, 2015; Rose & Abdel Rahman, 2016a, b; Schnur, 2014). While previous research has discussed the extent to which these two tasks may or may not reflect the same processes (Belke & Stielow, 2013), to our knowledge, this is the first study which provides a direct empirical comparison of semantic interference effects across these two tasks.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Blocked-cyclic and continuous naming tasks are used often interchangeably to draw inferences about the nature of semantic interference effects in naming and the processes that underlie speech production in general (Blocked Cyclic Naming: e.g., Belke, 2008, 2013; Damian & Als, 2005; de Zubicaray et al, 2014; Meinzer et al, 2016; Navarrete, Mahon, Lorenzoni, & Peressotti, 2016; Schnur et al, 2006, 2009; Continuous Naming: e.g., Belke, 2013; Canini et al, 2016; Howard et al, 2006; Navarrete et al, 2010; Ries et al, 2015; Rose & Abdel Rahman, 2016a, b; Schnur, 2014). While previous research has discussed the extent to which these two tasks may or may not reflect the same processes (Belke & Stielow, 2013), to our knowledge, this is the first study which provides a direct empirical comparison of semantic interference effects across these two tasks.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…However, all priming effects are not facilitatory, as naming pictures primed by semantically related items results in longer naming latencies (e.g., Brown, 1981). This semantic interference effect is thought to reflect the same long-lasting learning experience that facilitates naming (Oppenheim et al, 2010), since interference occurs regardless of whether semantically related pictures are presented consecutively (blocked/blocked cyclic naming; Abdel Rahman & Melinger, 2007, 2010; Belke, 2008; Belke, Meyer, & Damian, 2005; Damian & Als, 2005; Damian, Vigliocco, & Levelt, 2001; de Zubicaray, Johnson, Howard, & McMahon, 2014; Kroll & Stewart, 1994; Maess, Friederici, Damian, Meyer, & Levelt, 2002; Meinzer, Yetim, McMahon, & de Zubicaray, 2016; Navarrete, Del Prato, & Mahon, 2012; Schnur, Schwartz, Brecher, & Hodgson, 2006; Vigliocco, Lauer, Damian, & Levelt, 2002) or non-consecutively, with anywhere from two to eight intervening semantically unrelated items (i.e., continuous naming; e.g., Belke, 2013; Canini et al, 2016; Howard, Nickels, Coltheart, & Cole-Virtue, 2006; Navarrete, Mahon, & Caramazza, 2010; Runnqvist, Strijkers, Alario, & Costa, 2012; Schnur, 2014). The aim of this study was to test the assumptions of Oppenheim et al’s (2010) computational model of language production (henceforth, Dark Side Model) which implements both positive and negative effects of the “learning experience” in the same way when naming and successfully simulates naming performance in blocked cyclic and continuous naming.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Semantic control too, however, is sustained by a widespread set of anterior and posterior regions [23]. Arguably, this extensive anatomical involvement may be due to the diverse nature of the control processing, that may occur, for instance, at a lower computation level during the feature extraction phase [24] or higher up in the sequence of computations, when, for instance, semantic interference is addressed [25]. Different regions, then, may show some degree of specificity based on certain modalities (e.g., sensory, motor, or emotional), while the involvement of other areas spans across multiple modalities [26].…”
Section: The Cognitive and Neural Framework Behind Semantic Memorymentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Indeed, a functional imaging study by Canini et al . [70 ▪▪ ] implicates both the LIFG and basal ganglia in semantic control processes, showing increased activity of the LIFG and left caudate nucleus with cumulative semantic interference, the phenomenon of increasing naming latency after the presentation of categorically related/competitive items. This contribution of both the LIFG and basal ganglia in semantic selection processes is supported by diffusion tensor imaging studies, which demonstrate white matter connections between the two regions [71,72].…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%