1981
DOI: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.1981.tb03107.x
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Semantic Integration and the Development of Memory for Logical Inferences

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1

Citation Types

0
6
0

Year Published

1983
1983
2012
2012

Publication Types

Select...
5
1

Relationship

0
6

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 6 publications
(6 citation statements)
references
References 9 publications
0
6
0
Order By: Relevance
“…This effect was not seen in the 4-yearolds, who were able to discriminate between prototype and new items (recall that neither new nor prototype items were in the Acquisition phase). This effect persists when overall frequency (Table 2) and possible lexical memory effects are accounted for (Tables 1 and 3), thus controlling for the familiarity confound that has been a feature of other studies (for criticisms of this, see Liben & Posnansky, 1977;Paris & Mahoney, 1974;Small & Butterworth, 1981).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 97%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…This effect was not seen in the 4-yearolds, who were able to discriminate between prototype and new items (recall that neither new nor prototype items were in the Acquisition phase). This effect persists when overall frequency (Table 2) and possible lexical memory effects are accounted for (Tables 1 and 3), thus controlling for the familiarity confound that has been a feature of other studies (for criticisms of this, see Liben & Posnansky, 1977;Paris & Mahoney, 1974;Small & Butterworth, 1981).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 97%
“…Several investigators (Liben & Posnansky, 1977;Paris & Mahoney, 1974;Small & Butterworth, 1981) have pointed out that in most studies using verbal materials in the prototype-plus-distortion methodology, simple familiarity with the test sentences from previous presentations, rather than consistency with meaning or inference, is sufficient to account for false recognition of true sentences. To control for this methodological confound, it was important that the degree of lexical overlap that new and prototype (the critical comparison) sentences share with Acquisition items should be comparable.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Inferences are more likely in stories that are intact in a story grammar sense (e.g., Omanson et al, 1978). Finally, inferences are more likely if materials are structured so as to be compatible with world knowledge the child already possesses (e.g., Hildyard, 1979;Newcomb & Collins, 1979;Small & Butterworth, 1981). …”
Section: Going Beyond the Information Givenmentioning
confidence: 97%
“…Elementary school children in experiments by Markman (1977Markman ( , 1979 regularly failed to detect omissions and inconsistencies in instructions and prose passages, leading Markman (1981) to suggest that they tended to treat the individual statements of texts as isolated units instead of constructing integrated representations. Other investigators have shown that children between the ages of five and seven often do not integrate the information in a passage well enough to recognize implied relationships accurately (Liben and Posnansky, 1977;Moeser, 1976;Paris and Upton, 1976;Small and Butterworth, 1981), even when the separate items needed to make the correct inferences are available in memory (Collins and others, 1978;Schmidt and others, 1979). As a result, a young child who has heard a short descriptive passage, such as…”
Section: Children's Understanding Of Spoken Discoursementioning
confidence: 99%