2001
DOI: 10.1162/089892901564216
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Self-Organizing Dynamics of Lexical Access in Normals and Aphasics

Abstract: Abstract& The goal of this article is to illustrate the application of selforganizing dynamics in the design of a model of lexical access. We focus particularly on the mapping of sound structure on to the lexicon and the influence of that structure on lexical access. The approach is tested in a series of two sets of simulations that explicate how lexical access might occur in normal subjects and aphasic patients. Both sets of simulations address the behavioral effects of both phonological and phonetic variabil… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1

Citation Types

2
18
0

Year Published

2003
2003
2017
2017

Publication Types

Select...
5
1

Relationship

2
4

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 28 publications
(20 citation statements)
references
References 57 publications
(102 reference statements)
2
18
0
Order By: Relevance
“…These results suggest that activation of the lexicon is graded and that the degree of activation is influenced by the phonological distance of a nonword stimulus to a lexical entry. Consistent with these findings are behavioral and modeling data showing that lexical access is influenced by the degree to which the phonetic structure of the input matches word candidates (see McNellis & Blumstein, 2001;Luce & Pisoni, 1998). The similar areas of activation in the nonword condition compared to the related and unrelated conditions (Figure 1) can likely be attributed to the fact that these pseudowords automatically activate a set of phonologically similar lexical representations.…”
Section: Comparison Of Lexical Decision and Tone Taskssupporting
confidence: 62%
“…These results suggest that activation of the lexicon is graded and that the degree of activation is influenced by the phonological distance of a nonword stimulus to a lexical entry. Consistent with these findings are behavioral and modeling data showing that lexical access is influenced by the degree to which the phonetic structure of the input matches word candidates (see McNellis & Blumstein, 2001;Luce & Pisoni, 1998). The similar areas of activation in the nonword condition compared to the related and unrelated conditions (Figure 1) can likely be attributed to the fact that these pseudowords automatically activate a set of phonologically similar lexical representations.…”
Section: Comparison Of Lexical Decision and Tone Taskssupporting
confidence: 62%
“…Research results and models from auditory word recognition (cf. McNellis &Norris, 1994) should be combined with models based on tasks such as nonword repetition and serial recall (Gathercole & Baddeley, 1993;Martin & Ayala, 2004). Both research areas deal with language-impaired populations, the role of verbal short-term memory and spreading of activation within the lexical system.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Milberg, Blumstein, & Dworetzky, 1988;Misiurski, Blumstein, Rissman, & Berman, 2005;Utman, Blumstein, & Sullivan, 2001, but also see Baum, 1997;Gordon & Baum, 1994). Following Dell, Schwartz, Martin, Saffran, and Gagnon (1997) who modelled aphasic speech production errors by making global parameter adjustments to the 'normal' model, McNellis and Blumstein (2001) altered the parameter of lexical resting state to lower-than-normal for Broca's and to higher-than-normal for Wernicke's aphasics. McNellis and Blumstein showed that by such a simple parameter adjustment, experimental findings of Milberg et al (1988) and of Utman et al (2001) could be simulated.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…It is also proposed that the dynamics of activation across the neural systems underlying language vary as a function of the particular neural system, hence resulting in such clinical syndromes as Broca's and Wernicke's aphasia and in the different patterns of performance in on-line language processing tasks shown by these patients (cf. also McNellis & Blumstein, 1999).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%