2015
DOI: 10.1037/xhp0000041
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Self-organized complementary joint action: Behavioral dynamics of an interpersonal collision-avoidance task.

Abstract: Understanding stable patterns of interpersonal movement coordination is essential to understanding successful social interaction and activity (i.e., joint action). Previous research investigating such coordination has primarily focused on the synchronization of simple rhythmic movements (e.g., finger/forearm oscillations or pendulum swinging). Very few studies, however, have explored the stable patterns of coordination that emerge during task-directed complementary coordination tasks. Thus, the aim of the curr… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
3
2

Citation Types

4
69
0
1

Year Published

2016
2016
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
4
4

Relationship

0
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 56 publications
(81 citation statements)
references
References 63 publications
(95 reference statements)
4
69
0
1
Order By: Relevance
“…In line, similar coupling asymmetries have been demonstrated to prosper performance of complementary joint action like a collision-avoidance task (Richardson et al, 2015). Together, this may provide incentives for why “leader-follower” collaboration may be beneficial over perfectly balanced interpersonal interaction.…”
Section: Anisotropic Couplingsupporting
confidence: 54%
See 2 more Smart Citations
“…In line, similar coupling asymmetries have been demonstrated to prosper performance of complementary joint action like a collision-avoidance task (Richardson et al, 2015). Together, this may provide incentives for why “leader-follower” collaboration may be beneficial over perfectly balanced interpersonal interaction.…”
Section: Anisotropic Couplingsupporting
confidence: 54%
“…The latter also draws in an explicit role division: the stroke rower sets the pace for the other rower(s) to adhere to (de Poel et al, 2016). Recently, researchers have started to examine such interactional directionalities in between-person settings, mainly in context of leader-follower relations (e.g., Konvalinka et al, 2010; Vesper and Richardson, 2014), such as in the context of a “mirror game” (Noy et al, 2011; Słowiński et al, 2016), of which some studies specifically pertained to (or referred to) a dynamic model of anisotropic/asymmetric coupling (Varlet et al, 2012; Meerhoff and de Poel, 2014; Fine, 2015; Richardson et al, 2015). Importantly, between-person coordination typically entails bidirectional “leader-follower” interaction rather than strict unidirectional “master-slave” dependency (e.g., Meerhoff and de Poel, 2014).…”
Section: Anisotropic Couplingmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…For example, dyads were asked to move dots on a screen together, so that one participant had to move a dot back and forth from the lower-left to the upper-right and the other one from the lower-right to the upper-left [17]. However, when the dots crashed into each other during these oscillatory movements, the trial was counted as a failure.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…This similitude principle indicates that the dynamic-systems approach can account for interactions at different behavioral levels, independent of the nature of the connections between the system’s components (i.e., neural, mechanical or informational). Whereas most of the studies addressing the dynamics of joint actions concerned non-functional or stereotyped oscillatory limb or whole-body movements (such as swinging legs or rocking chairs together, Schmidt et al, 1990; Richardson et al, 2007), a few studies have shown that the interactive behavior of two individuals can also account for the observed coordinated patterns in more goal-directed tasks (Mottet et al, 2001; Richardson et al, 2015; Romero et al, 2015). …”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%