2013
DOI: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.04.051
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Selectivity for large nonmanipulable objects in scene-selective visual cortex does not require visual experience

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
3
1

Citation Types

14
107
2

Year Published

2015
2015
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
7

Relationship

2
5

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 103 publications
(126 citation statements)
references
References 44 publications
14
107
2
Order By: Relevance
“…However, in future studies we shall try to parametrize and thus model other visual as well as non-visual sensory properties implied by nouns (e.g., shape, 22 sound) in order to investigate the degree of segregation across sensory regions of these properties. Concerning the anatomy of the real-word size effect, previous literature has shown the implication of lateral-occipital, inferotemporal, and parahippocampal cortices (Konkle and Oliva, 2012;He et al, 2013). The discrepancy between those studies and the current one can be traced down to the numerous methodological differences:…”
Section: Implied Real-world Size Information In Primary Visual Areasmentioning
confidence: 66%
See 2 more Smart Citations
“…However, in future studies we shall try to parametrize and thus model other visual as well as non-visual sensory properties implied by nouns (e.g., shape, 22 sound) in order to investigate the degree of segregation across sensory regions of these properties. Concerning the anatomy of the real-word size effect, previous literature has shown the implication of lateral-occipital, inferotemporal, and parahippocampal cortices (Konkle and Oliva, 2012;He et al, 2013). The discrepancy between those studies and the current one can be traced down to the numerous methodological differences:…”
Section: Implied Real-world Size Information In Primary Visual Areasmentioning
confidence: 66%
“…When they did not use pictures, but words, as we do, they engaged subejcts in tasks involving active size comparison (He et al, 2013) or imagery of objets in their prototypical or atypical size (Konkle and Oliva, 2012 studies 3), thus drawing subject's attention on the size dimension. Instead, in our experiment, subjects were asked to actively think of the whole concept referred to by the words, with no specific focus on the size dimension;…”
Section: Implied Real-world Size Information In Primary Visual Areasmentioning
confidence: 77%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…As such, it is unsurprising that functional neuroimaging techniques have revealed a putative “core” scene processing network in the human brain that responds strongly when viewing navigationally relevant stimuli (e.g., scenes) versus other visual categories. This core network is thought to include posterior parahippocampal gyrus [PHG; Aguirre et al, 1998a; Epstein et al, 2003; Epstein and Kanwisher, 1998], retrosplenial cortex [RSC; Auger et al, 2012; Epstein et al, 2007; Vann et al, 2009] and the transverse occipital sulcus [TOS; Dilks et al, 2013; Ganaden et al, 2013; He et al, 2013; Mullin and Steeves, 2011; Nasr et al, 2011]. As seen in the neural processing of other visual categories [Taylor and Downing, 2011], these regions appear to support distinct but complementary aspects of scene processing, and are differentially modulated by changes in viewpoint [Epstein et al, 2003, 2007; Park and Chun, 2009], spatial layout [Harel et al, 2013; Park et al, 2015], and lower‐level spatial features [Kravitz et al, 2011a; Nasr et al, 2014].…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Interestingly, some evidence points toward a large-scale categorical sensitivity and layout of ventral visual cortex in this population. For instance, He and colleagues (14) observed small-vs.-large object selectivity in parahippocampal regions. Moreover, the medial-to-lateral bias that is associated with processing the animate-inanimate distinction is found in blind individuals (15).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%