2016
DOI: 10.3758/s13414-016-1127-2
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Selective attention modulates the effect of target location probability on redundant signal processing

Abstract: We investigated the decision process underlying the detection of targets at multiple locations. In three experiments using the same observers, target location probability and attentional instructions were manipulated. A redundanttarget detection task was conducted in which participants were required to detect a dot presented at one of two locations. When the dot appeared at the two locations with equal frequency (Experiment 1), those participants who were found to have limited to unlimited capacity were shown … Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

0
5
0

Year Published

2017
2017
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
5
2

Relationship

2
5

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 7 publications
(5 citation statements)
references
References 80 publications
(125 reference statements)
0
5
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Experiment 3 included an awareness test that differs substantially from the rest of the experiments included in the meta-analysis (Criterion 2). Chang, Little, and Yang (2016) Experiments 2 and 3 did not include an awareness test (Criterion 2) and there was an obvious explicit component (Criterion 3).…”
Section: Appendix Amentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Experiment 3 included an awareness test that differs substantially from the rest of the experiments included in the meta-analysis (Criterion 2). Chang, Little, and Yang (2016) Experiments 2 and 3 did not include an awareness test (Criterion 2) and there was an obvious explicit component (Criterion 3).…”
Section: Appendix Amentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Hull's "law of less work"; Hull, 1943;Kool, McGuire, Rosen, & Botvinick, 2010). Indeed, across many perceptual decision-making experiments, we have not observed exhaustive processing when the experiment allowed for self-termination (Blunden et al, 2020(Blunden et al, , 2015Chang, Little, & Yang, 2016;Cheng, McCarthy, Wang, Palmeri, & Little, 2018;Little et al, 2011Little et al, , 2013Moneer et al, 2016; C.-T. Yang, Altieri, & Little, 2018;C.-T. Yang, Little, & Hsu, 2014;H. Yang, Little, Eidels, & Townsend, 2019).…”
Section: Differentiating Response Time Architecturesmentioning
confidence: 72%
“…This follows from the idea that observers act to conserve mental effort (e.g., Hull’s “law of less work”; Hull, 1943; Kool et al, 2010). Indeed, across many perceptual decision-making experiments, we have not observed exhaustive processing when the experiment allowed for self-termination (Blunden et al, 2015, 2020; Chang et al, 2016; Cheng et al, 2018; Fific et al, 2010; Little et al, 2011, 2013; Moneer et al, 2016; Yang et al, 2014, 2018, 2021). Further, there is also evidence that participants will attempt to apply self-terminating processing even in exhaustive tasks which do not permit self-termination without error (Bushmakin et al, 2015; Howard et al, 2021).…”
Section: First- Versus Second-order Integrationmentioning
confidence: 92%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…The notable take-home message is that we did not find any evidence for coactivity in the processing of own-race faces, contrary to the strong-holistic-encoding hypothesis. Serial processing, often as a result of controlled attention (Chang, Little, & Yang, 2016;Shiffrin & Schneider, 1977), may operate more slowly than more automatic, parallel processing. Hence, parallel processing of own-race face features is sufficient to explain the own-race processing benefit that has been seen in several studies.…”
Section: Resultsmentioning
confidence: 99%