2013
DOI: 10.1037/a0030678
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Selecting valuable information to remember: Age-related differences and similarities in self-regulated learning.

Abstract: It is often necessary to selectively attend to important information, at the expense of less important information, especially if you know you cannot remember large amounts of information. The present study examined how younger and older adults select valuable information to study, when given unrestricted choices about how to allocate study time. Participants were shown a display of point values ranging from 1-30. Participants could choose which values to study, and the associated word was then shown. Study ti… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1

Citation Types

15
97
1
5

Year Published

2014
2014
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
6
2
1

Relationship

2
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 106 publications
(118 citation statements)
references
References 43 publications
(77 reference statements)
15
97
1
5
Order By: Relevance
“…These effects have been shown using a variety of memory tests including free and serial recall, and recognition memory (Adcock et al, 2006;Eysenck & Eysenck, 1982;Harley, 1965;Loftus & Wickens, 1970;Madan, Caplan, Lau, & Fujiwara, 2012;Spaniol, Schain, & Bowen, 2013;Wolosin, Zeithamova, & Preston, 2012). Furthermore, these findings extend to value-based learning where points are used instead of monetary incentives (Ariel & Castel, 2014;Castel, 2007;Castel, Benjamin, Craik, & Watkins, 2002;Castel, Murayama, Friedman, McGillivray, & Link, 2013;Friedman & Castel, 2011;Soderstrom & McCabe, 2011). Taken together these findings suggest that people are acting to maximize reward and are able to allocate cognitive resources during learning so that higher value items are better encoded than lower value items.…”
supporting
confidence: 55%
“…These effects have been shown using a variety of memory tests including free and serial recall, and recognition memory (Adcock et al, 2006;Eysenck & Eysenck, 1982;Harley, 1965;Loftus & Wickens, 1970;Madan, Caplan, Lau, & Fujiwara, 2012;Spaniol, Schain, & Bowen, 2013;Wolosin, Zeithamova, & Preston, 2012). Furthermore, these findings extend to value-based learning where points are used instead of monetary incentives (Ariel & Castel, 2014;Castel, 2007;Castel, Benjamin, Craik, & Watkins, 2002;Castel, Murayama, Friedman, McGillivray, & Link, 2013;Friedman & Castel, 2011;Soderstrom & McCabe, 2011). Taken together these findings suggest that people are acting to maximize reward and are able to allocate cognitive resources during learning so that higher value items are better encoded than lower value items.…”
supporting
confidence: 55%
“…Prior experiments have, however, demonstrated value-directed remembering without explicitly referring to item value as it related to recall goals (Friedman et al, 2015) or to high-value items specifically (Castel, Lee, Humphreys, & Moore, 2011;Castel, Murayama, Friedman, McGillivray, & Link, 2013). Making the instructions less explicit might impact List 1 performance, in that participants could otherwise overestimate their ability to remember all of the items and thus not immediately take item value into account, but it is not thought that the instructions alone account for the overall performance or selectivity improvements.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…This selectivity is particularly apparent within tasks that explicitly vary the objective value of the to-be-remembered information. According to a “value-directed remembering” framework (Castel, 2008; Castel, McGillivray, & Friedman, 2012), older adults can remember higher-value information as well as younger adults by selectively prioritizing and encoding this information over less important information (Castel, Benjamin, Craik, & Watkins, 2002; Castel et al, 2013). Furthermore, McGillivray and Castel (2011) demonstrated that both younger and older adults can learn to selectively choose to focus on and remember high value items when there are consequences associated with the failure to recall this higher value information.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%