2014
DOI: 10.1007/s00221-014-3951-8
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Segment interdependency and gaze anchoring during manual two-segment sequences

Abstract: This study examined two-segment pointing movements with various accuracy constraints to test whether there is segment interdependency in saccadic eye movements that accompany manual actions. The other purpose was to examine how planning of movement accuracy and amplitude for the second pointing influences the timing of gaze shift to the second target at the transition between two segments. Participants performed a rapid two-segment pointing task, in which the first segment had two target sizes, and the second … Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
3
1
1

Citation Types

0
11
0

Year Published

2016
2016
2022
2022

Publication Types

Select...
7

Relationship

1
6

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 15 publications
(11 citation statements)
references
References 53 publications
0
11
0
Order By: Relevance
“…According to the optimized sub‐movement model, the primary sub‐movement of a rapid aiming movement is under central control, and the secondary sub‐movement relies upon visual feedback (Meyer, Abrams, Kornblum, Wright, & Smith, ). In a two‐segment movement task, vision guides the hand to the target, verifies the movement's completion and prepares the movement to the next target (Rand, ). However, the prefrontal cortex and its connections with the cortical and subcortical regions, which are thought to play essential roles in top–down cognitive control, continue to develop during the first two decades of life (Gogtay et al, ; Hwang et al, ; Mills et al, ; Somerville et al, ).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 2 more Smart Citations
“…According to the optimized sub‐movement model, the primary sub‐movement of a rapid aiming movement is under central control, and the secondary sub‐movement relies upon visual feedback (Meyer, Abrams, Kornblum, Wright, & Smith, ). In a two‐segment movement task, vision guides the hand to the target, verifies the movement's completion and prepares the movement to the next target (Rand, ). However, the prefrontal cortex and its connections with the cortical and subcortical regions, which are thought to play essential roles in top–down cognitive control, continue to develop during the first two decades of life (Gogtay et al, ; Hwang et al, ; Mills et al, ; Somerville et al, ).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…It has been observed that changing the difficulty of one movement segment influences the entire movement sequence, which indicates that our central nervous system accounts for the features of both segments in the planning or organisation of a movement sequence (Adam et al, ; Rand et al, ). Moreover, planning of the entire movement sequence becomes more difficult if the first movement segment is more difficult (Rand, ) or if the participants are older (Rand & Stelmach, ). Childhood is a critical period for brain development, but few studies have examined the control and learning of two‐segment movements in children.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Eye movements were differentiated by using a two-point central-difference algorithm to obtain velocity. Onset and offset of the initial saccade immediately after the target presentation were determined using a threshold criterion of 20 cm/s in the velocity profile [8,11,12]. The results of this automatic procedure were inspected and corrected manually as needed based on visual inspection; 27.7% (onset) and 22.8% (offset) of all analyzed trials were corrected.…”
Section: Methodsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…This gaze behavior (called gaze anchoring [10]) is useful to control reaching movement, especially during the homing-in phase, because it places the visual target in fovea as the hand approaches to the target, while temporally removing the added burden of spatial updating for gaze shift [13]. Functionally, it allows for effective use of visual feedback of both the target and the approaching hand to guide and complete a precise reaching movement [11,12,14,15]. …”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%