1988
DOI: 10.1016/0165-5876(88)90071-7
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Secretory otitis media and language development: a six-year follow-up study with case-control

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

1
14
1

Year Published

1990
1990
2008
2008

Publication Types

Select...
8

Relationship

1
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 35 publications
(16 citation statements)
references
References 21 publications
1
14
1
Order By: Relevance
“…As in some other longitudinal studies of broader linguistic and educational outcome measures in OME (Lous, Fiellau-Nikolajsen, & Jeppesen, 1988;Roberts, Burchinal, Davis, Collier, & Henderson, 1991), this relationship was no longer evident at school age (Schilder, van Manen, Zielhuis, Grievink, Peters, & van den Broek, 1993a). These findings support the hypothesis developed by Feagans (1986) that children may recover their basic language skills when otitis media becomes less frequent in later years.…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 81%
“…As in some other longitudinal studies of broader linguistic and educational outcome measures in OME (Lous, Fiellau-Nikolajsen, & Jeppesen, 1988;Roberts, Burchinal, Davis, Collier, & Henderson, 1991), this relationship was no longer evident at school age (Schilder, van Manen, Zielhuis, Grievink, Peters, & van den Broek, 1993a). These findings support the hypothesis developed by Feagans (1986) that children may recover their basic language skills when otitis media becomes less frequent in later years.…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 81%
“…This position changed radically with the publication of population‐based cohort studies with high recruitment and retention rates and excellent ascertainment of both the condition and its postulated outcomes. In general, these studies have not demonstrated significant adverse language or academic outcomes for persistent OME either in the preschool or the elementary school years 19−22 . Acknowledging that OME is a different condition, and therefore may have different impacts from sensorineural losses of similar magnitude, this nonetheless raises a challenge: 100 000 Australian school‐aged children are likely to have slight or mild sensorineural hearing loss 8 , and they deserve research of equal quality.…”
Section: Research Directionsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Thus, we excluded studies 5,[33][34][35][36][37][38] in meta-analyses that were in an age grouping that would contain data from only 1 or 2 cohorts (eg, only 2 correlation studies examined OME and receptive language in infancy). Eleven age groupings remained with at least 3 studies on a particular outcome and were available for meta-analyses: 1) infancy: receptive language versus hearing loss (correlation studies); 2) infancy: expressive language versus hearing loss (correlation studies); 3) preschool: receptive language versus OME (correlation studies); 4) preschool: receptive language versus OME (group studies); 5) preschool: expressive language versus OME (correlation studies); 6) preschool: expressive language versus OME (group studies); 7) preschool: vocabulary comprehension (Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test [PPVT]) 39 versus OME (correlation studies); 8) preschool: vocabulary comprehension (PPVT) 39 versus OME (group studies); 9) preschool: expressive vocabulary (number of different words [NDW]) versus OME (correlation studies); 10) preschool: expressive syntax (mean length of utterance [MLU]) versus OME (correlation studies); and 11) preschool: speech versus OME (group studies).…”
Section: Study Selectionmentioning
confidence: 99%