2019
DOI: 10.3389/fnins.2019.00523
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Scene Regularity Interacts With Individual Biases to Modulate Perceptual Stability

Abstract: Sensory input is inherently ambiguous but our brains achieve remarkable perceptual stability. Prior experience and knowledge of the statistical properties of the world are thought to play a key role in the stabilization process. Individual differences in responses to ambiguous input and biases toward one or the other interpretation could modulate the decision mechanism for perception. However, the role of perceptual bias and its interaction with stimulus spatial properties such as regularity and element densit… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
3
1

Citation Types

3
1
0

Year Published

2020
2020
2022
2022

Publication Types

Select...
4
1

Relationship

1
4

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 5 publications
(4 citation statements)
references
References 46 publications
(76 reference statements)
3
1
0
Order By: Relevance
“…For example, in a task sequence where subjects first had to make a categorical judgment about a stimulus feature before providing an estimate of the feature value (motion direction [ 7 9 ], orientation [ 10 , 11 ] or numerosity [ 9 , 12 , 13 ]), the resulting estimates showed systematic biases in favor of their preceding category choices. Similar bias patterns have also been found in sequential tasks that did not explicitly involve categorical judgments [ 8 , 14 17 ] or estimates [ 18 , 19 ], suggesting that they are a wide-spread and general phenomenon in hierarchical perceptual inference [ 20 ]. A general theory proposes that these bias patterns emerge because inference is intrinsically a top-down process where low-level feature estimates are conditioned on the observer’s preceding, (explicit or implicit) high-level categorical judgment [ 10 , 16 , 20 22 ].…”
Section: Introductionsupporting
confidence: 58%
“…For example, in a task sequence where subjects first had to make a categorical judgment about a stimulus feature before providing an estimate of the feature value (motion direction [ 7 9 ], orientation [ 10 , 11 ] or numerosity [ 9 , 12 , 13 ]), the resulting estimates showed systematic biases in favor of their preceding category choices. Similar bias patterns have also been found in sequential tasks that did not explicitly involve categorical judgments [ 8 , 14 17 ] or estimates [ 18 , 19 ], suggesting that they are a wide-spread and general phenomenon in hierarchical perceptual inference [ 20 ]. A general theory proposes that these bias patterns emerge because inference is intrinsically a top-down process where low-level feature estimates are conditioned on the observer’s preceding, (explicit or implicit) high-level categorical judgment [ 10 , 16 , 20 22 ].…”
Section: Introductionsupporting
confidence: 58%
“…Similar bias patterns have also been found in sequential tasks that did not explicitly involve categorical judgments 13,14,8,15,16 or estimates 17,18 , suggesting that these biases are a wide-spread and general phenomenon in hierarchical perceptual inference 19 . A general theory proposes that these bias patterns emerge because inference is intrinsically a top-down process where low-level feature estimates are conditioned on the observer's preceding, (explicit or implicit) high-level categorical judgment 20,21,15 .…”
Section: Introductionsupporting
confidence: 65%
“…Behaviorally these two paradigms share a number of important features, notably that perception switches between a single coherent stream to segregated streams of information. Both paradigms also tend to follow a similar initial response pattern where the first perceptual phase is integrated for a relatively long time, followed by switching back-and-forth between shorter duration segregated and integrated percepts, supporting the hypothesis that the integrated percept serves as the default, or baseline, given stimulus parameters that elicit bistable perception (Hupé and Rubin, 2003;Pressnitzer and Hupé, 2006;Li et al, 2019). Additional hallmarks of spontaneous bistable perception, such as unpredictability from one perceptual phase to the next, and observance of logarithmic distribution of phase durations, are also present in both the ABA and moving plaid streaming paradigms (Pressnitzer and Hupé, 2006;Carter et al, 2014;Denham et al, 2018).…”
Section: Introductionsupporting
confidence: 57%