2008
DOI: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2007.12.001
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Salivary cortisol, stress and mood in healthy older adults: The Zenith study

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

2
29
0

Year Published

2009
2009
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
7

Relationship

0
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 44 publications
(33 citation statements)
references
References 78 publications
2
29
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Effects were also independent of psychological distress, suggesting that the association was not simply due to the absence of negative affect. A similar pattern of association was reported by Simpson et al (Simpson, McConville et al 2008); cortisol levels in the early afternoon and late evening were measured in 41 older adults (mean age 61 years) for 7 days, and the average afternoon and evening values calculated. Momentary measures of mood were also collected four times a day, on rising, in the early afternoon, in the early evening and in the late evening, and these measures were also aggregated.…”
Section: Cortisolsupporting
confidence: 84%
“…Effects were also independent of psychological distress, suggesting that the association was not simply due to the absence of negative affect. A similar pattern of association was reported by Simpson et al (Simpson, McConville et al 2008); cortisol levels in the early afternoon and late evening were measured in 41 older adults (mean age 61 years) for 7 days, and the average afternoon and evening values calculated. Momentary measures of mood were also collected four times a day, on rising, in the early afternoon, in the early evening and in the late evening, and these measures were also aggregated.…”
Section: Cortisolsupporting
confidence: 84%
“…The absence of a main effect of time of assessment for positive affect may suggest an effect of emotional regulation in both younger and older adults' stress levels, because negative affect increased as a result of the stressor, which is associated with perceived stress (Simpson et al, 2008), whereas positive affect remained at the same level per age group. The higher levels of positive affect in the older relative to the younger group may reflect better emotional well-being in older than younger adults regardless of the situation they are in (Carstensen, 1995).…”
Section: Psychological Mood Measures and Workloadmentioning
confidence: 65%
“…If perceived stress increased, positive affect should decrease (cf. Simpson et al, 2008) unless participants were determined to maintain their level of alertness and enthusiasm despite the effects of the stressor that were already demonstrated with the physiological, negative mood and workload measures.…”
Section: Psychological Mood Measures and Workloadmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Interestingly, UFC was not related to perceived stress over the previous month (PSS score), the amount of stress encountered on the day of the UFC measurement (DSI score), or to concern related to eating and body image (as assessed by dietary restraint). Several other groups report no relationship between salivary cortisol and PSS scores among young women leaving the welfare system [34], undergraduate students [21,35], white-collared male workers [36,37], post-menopausal women [20], or healthy middle-aged [38] and older adults [39]. The effect of perceived stress on cortisol may be mediated by other psychosocial variables, such as mood [37,39], which were beyond the scope of this study.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 91%