Salience in Second Language Acquisition 2017
DOI: 10.4324/9781315399027-1
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Salience in Second Language Acquisition and Related Fields

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

1
14
0
1

Year Published

2020
2020
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
6

Relationship

0
6

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 13 publications
(16 citation statements)
references
References 1 publication
1
14
0
1
Order By: Relevance
“…Indeed, in the present study, we observed a significantly higher skipping rate under the unenhanced condition. Other factors that might have contributed to the more positive outcomes for textual enhancement in the captioning studies include prior knowledge (e.g., Han et al, 2008;Park, 2004) and the relative salience of the targeted grammatical constructions (Gass et al, 2017). Both Lee and Révész (2018) and the present experiment targeted a perceptually salient construction, of which learners had some prior knowledge.…”
Section: Captioning and Development In L2 Grammatical Knowledge (Rq1)mentioning
confidence: 88%
“…Indeed, in the present study, we observed a significantly higher skipping rate under the unenhanced condition. Other factors that might have contributed to the more positive outcomes for textual enhancement in the captioning studies include prior knowledge (e.g., Han et al, 2008;Park, 2004) and the relative salience of the targeted grammatical constructions (Gass et al, 2017). Both Lee and Révész (2018) and the present experiment targeted a perceptually salient construction, of which learners had some prior knowledge.…”
Section: Captioning and Development In L2 Grammatical Knowledge (Rq1)mentioning
confidence: 88%
“…In the L2 acquisition context, salience has been defined as “how noticeable or explicit a linguistic structure is in the input” (Loewen & Reinders, 2011, p. 152), with salience making items easy to perceive (Gass, Spinner, & Behney, 2018). The construct of salience has been approached from two main angles, perceptual salience and constructed salience (Gass et al., 2018). Perceptual salience refers to linguistic features that are intrinsically more noticeable.…”
Section: Background Literaturementioning
confidence: 99%
“…For example, longer morphological markers are more salient than shorter ones and are therefore easier to detect (Behney, Spinner, Gass, & Valmori, 2018). Constructed salience “occurs when some outside source creates a context for some feature to become prominent” and leads learners to notice and potentially process that particular feature (Gass et al., 2018, p. 7). Different methods to achieve constructed salience have been employed in vocabulary research, with the majority of studies focusing on the role of input enhancement (e.g., Barcroft, 2003; Kim, 2006) and input flooding (e.g., Webb, Newton, & Chang, 2013).…”
Section: Background Literaturementioning
confidence: 99%
“…The second implication of our findings that we wish to discuss is the fact that, if the ILH is defensible, we should expect to find learners with systems that have covert contrasts. The ILH makes the claim that all L2 learners internalize a system that enables them to speak and understand utterances in the TL, and that this system can be independent of both the learner’s NL and the TL (Gass, Behney & Plonsky, 2013; Tarone, (2006). An IL system with a covert contrast is independent of both the NL and TL in that it is not NL-like, because the NL lacks the contrast, and it is not TL like, because the contrast is implemented in a way that is not perceived by native speakers of the TL.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%