2016
DOI: 10.1016/j.cognition.2016.04.014
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Robust representation of shape in a Grey parrot (Psittacus erithacus)

Abstract: A Grey parrot, Griffin (Psittacus erithacus), previously taught English labels for various colors and shapes with respect to three-dimensional (3D) stimuli, was tested on his ability to transfer to very different two-dimensional (2D) images consisting of modal and amodal completion stimuli. For modal completion (aka subjective contours), Kanizsa figures were constructed using black 'pac-men' to form regular polygons on colored paper. For amodal completion, portions of variously colored regular 2D polygons were… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

1
52
0

Year Published

2017
2017
2022
2022

Publication Types

Select...
5
3

Relationship

2
6

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 56 publications
(55 citation statements)
references
References 82 publications
(104 reference statements)
1
52
0
Order By: Relevance
“…As had been the case in other studies with Griffin (e.g., Pepperberg & Nakayama, 2016) and another Grey parrot, Alex (e.g., Pepperberg & Carey, 2012), Griffin occasionally engaged in noncompliant behaviour. These instances were considered mistrials and were not included in our data set.…”
Section: Noncompliance Procedures/mistrialssupporting
confidence: 71%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…As had been the case in other studies with Griffin (e.g., Pepperberg & Nakayama, 2016) and another Grey parrot, Alex (e.g., Pepperberg & Carey, 2012), Griffin occasionally engaged in noncompliant behaviour. These instances were considered mistrials and were not included in our data set.…”
Section: Noncompliance Procedures/mistrialssupporting
confidence: 71%
“…However, not only might Griffin's training regime exercise and develop executive function, it also might specifically exercise and develop the logical capacities at issue in these studies. Griffin experiences very few trials tapping any probed construct in any one day (e.g., Pepperberg & Nakayama, 2016), and so he is never rewarded for adopting a strategy that yields "enough" rewards. Rather, he must provide a single answer, and there is a high premium on figuring out a strategy that is certain to succeed in any given context.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Indeed, many animals are trained to cooperate with research (e.g. hydro-dynamic trail following in seals: Dehnhardt et al, 2001; underwater visual acuity in manatees: Bauer et al, 2003; shape representation in a grey parrot: Pepperberg and Nakayama, 2016). Animals are increasingly being tested individually in purpose-built research centres within zoos (e.g.…”
Section: The Use Of Animals In Researchmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…However, I now am not entirely sure about a corollary of those two claimsthat acquisition of symbolic representation does not affect how subjects manipulate information-I believe that, in some specific instances, such a change in processing ability may exist. Possibly, once a non-human understands that a symbol can be used to represent an object or an action, it can then understand how, for example, a three-dimensional entity can be represented by a two-dimensional one (e.g., optical illusions, Pepperberg and Nakayama, 2016) or that two symbols (e.g., one vocal and one visual) that separately represent the same object can then represent each other (a formal equivalence, Pepperberg, 2006b), or that symbols can be used as place-markers to assist in tasks requiring memory and evaluation of probability (Pepperberg and Pailian, 2017;Clements et al, 2018). I will discuss one such topic-various studies on numerical concepts-in depth.…”
Section: The Role Of Languagementioning
confidence: 99%