2000
DOI: 10.1007/bf03395353
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Risk Aversion in Human Subjects under Conditions of Probabilistic Reward

Abstract: This study examined the role of contingencies and experimental context in human decision making. Twelve subjects participated across a series of conditions that provided response alternatives of a small, high-probability reinforcer (non-risky alternative) or a larger, low-probability reinforcer (risky alternative). A range of reinforcer amounts and probabilities were used in a discrete trial design with repeated trials across multiple sessions. The conditions of this study more closely modeled studies with non… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
2
1

Citation Types

0
5
0

Year Published

2000
2000
2015
2015

Publication Types

Select...
6

Relationship

3
3

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 6 publications
(5 citation statements)
references
References 20 publications
0
5
0
Order By: Relevance
“…The risk sensitivity demonstrated here is in accord with the results of recent earningsbudget studies with human subjects (e.g., Pietras & Hackenberg, 2001;Rode et al, 1999), but contrasts with most previous risky-choice research with humans. Prior studies have shown across a range of conditions that choice is consistently risk averse, whether the outcomes are fixed and variable reinforcer amounts or fixed and variable reinforcer delays (e.g., Kohn, Kohn, & Staddon, 1992), or whether the outcomes are hypothetical and presented verbally (e.g., Rachlin, Raineri, & Cross, 1991;Schneider & Lopes, 1986), or real and directly experienced (e.g., Lane & Cherek, 2000;Schmitt & Whitmeyer, 1990).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The risk sensitivity demonstrated here is in accord with the results of recent earningsbudget studies with human subjects (e.g., Pietras & Hackenberg, 2001;Rode et al, 1999), but contrasts with most previous risky-choice research with humans. Prior studies have shown across a range of conditions that choice is consistently risk averse, whether the outcomes are fixed and variable reinforcer amounts or fixed and variable reinforcer delays (e.g., Kohn, Kohn, & Staddon, 1992), or whether the outcomes are hypothetical and presented verbally (e.g., Rachlin, Raineri, & Cross, 1991;Schneider & Lopes, 1986), or real and directly experienced (e.g., Lane & Cherek, 2000;Schmitt & Whitmeyer, 1990).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…A third possibility is that printing a monetary outcome on screen (+$0.50 or −$0.20) following each risky choice and a nonmonetary outcome (“Time Out”) following each nonrisky choice differentially affected participants' response preferences. Prior studies in our lab have revealed that participants treated “$0.00” as an aversive stimulus and would respond to avoid it (Lane & Cherek, 2000c). Therefore, the “Time Out” message was selected to avoid the possible confound of both choices being associated with an aversive outcome.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The task, developed across prior studies in our laboratory (Lane & Cherek, 2000a, 2000c), was designed to measure individual patterns of risk-taking behavior. The task presented discrete trials in which the participant chose between two response options, labeled C and A on the response panel (Button B had no function).…”
Section: Methodsmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Many procedural features may affect choices between risky and non-risky alternatives. Notably, there may be differences in risktaking behavior when outcomes are real rather than hypothetical (Slovic 1969;Rachlin and Frankel 1969;Lane and Cherek 2000a). And risk-taking tendencies may be altered when the task in question (e.g.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 98%