2003
DOI: 10.1093/deafed/eng014
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Rhyme Generation in Deaf Students: The Effect of Exposure to Cued Speech

Abstract: ).This study compares the rhyme-generation ability of deaf participants with severe to profound hearing losses from cued speech (CS) and non-cued speech (NCS) backgrounds with a hearing comparison group for consistent orthographyto-phonology (O-P) rhyming elements, or rimes (e.g., -ail in sail is always pronounced the same), and inconsistent orthography-to-phonology (I-O-P) rhyming elements where the orthographic rime (e.g., -ear) has different pronunciations in words such as bear and rear. Rhyming accuracy wa… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1

Citation Types

0
30
0
1

Year Published

2006
2006
2017
2017

Publication Types

Select...
8
1
1

Relationship

0
10

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 58 publications
(34 citation statements)
references
References 23 publications
(60 reference statements)
0
30
0
1
Order By: Relevance
“…The empirical evidence collected in English, French, Spanish, Farsi and even Amharic (national language of Ethiopia) shows that congenitally deaf children who were exposed to CS from their earliest months by their parents and other caregivers can reach levels of mastery of spoken (phonology, lexical, morpho-syntactic) language and written language (word reading, reading comprehension, spelling) within range of age-matched hearing peers when tested at school age. Children with late and less intensive exposure (i.e., at the age of 5–6 years, and at school only) do not demonstrate the outstanding phonological and reading abilities of the early CS-users, confirming the existence of a sensitive period for language acquisition via the visual modality (Nicholls and McGill, 1982; Périer et al, 1990; Charlier and Leybaert, 2000; Leybaert, 2000; LaSasso et al, 2003; Torres and Moreno-Torres, 2006; Koo et al, 2008; LaSasso, 2010; Movallali, 2011; Heracleous et al, 2012; Colin et al, 2013; Rees and Bladel, 2013; Shull et al, 2016). …”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 91%
“…The empirical evidence collected in English, French, Spanish, Farsi and even Amharic (national language of Ethiopia) shows that congenitally deaf children who were exposed to CS from their earliest months by their parents and other caregivers can reach levels of mastery of spoken (phonology, lexical, morpho-syntactic) language and written language (word reading, reading comprehension, spelling) within range of age-matched hearing peers when tested at school age. Children with late and less intensive exposure (i.e., at the age of 5–6 years, and at school only) do not demonstrate the outstanding phonological and reading abilities of the early CS-users, confirming the existence of a sensitive period for language acquisition via the visual modality (Nicholls and McGill, 1982; Périer et al, 1990; Charlier and Leybaert, 2000; Leybaert, 2000; LaSasso et al, 2003; Torres and Moreno-Torres, 2006; Koo et al, 2008; LaSasso, 2010; Movallali, 2011; Heracleous et al, 2012; Colin et al, 2013; Rees and Bladel, 2013; Shull et al, 2016). …”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 91%
“…Focused instruction in the use of phonics has also produced consistently positive reading outcomes for hearing children (McGuinness, 1997). While there is much less comparable research in deaf education, the evaluation of speech-based visual phonics programs for deaf children have produced promising outcomes with respect to the development of their phonological awareness and reading comprehension skills (LaSasso, Crain, & Leybaert, 2003;Leybaert, Alegria, Hage, & Charlier, 1998;Narr, 2008). However, there is a realization that profoundly deaf children cannot rely on their spoken language skills when they begin to learn to read (Hermans, Knoors, Ormel, & Verhoeven, 2007).…”
Section: Literacy Needs Of Deaf Adults In Australiamentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Research into the phonological awareness and development of component reading skills of deaf children indicate that those with early and consistent exposure to cueing develop a phonological representation of words in their language (LaSasso, Crain, & Leybaert, 2003), and can learn phonic generalizations for spelling in the same way as hearing children who speak the language (Alegria, Dejean, Capouillez & Leybaert, 1990;Leybaert & Charlier, 1996;Leybaert & Lechat, 2001). Deaf children with early and consistent exposure to cueing at home and at school have demonstrated word coding, memory, and reading abilities similar to hearing peers, and superior to deaf children from other communication backgrounds (Leybaert & Charlier, 1996;Wandel, 1989).…”
Section: Research Into Cued Speechmentioning
confidence: 99%