2006
DOI: 10.1007/s10044-006-0054-6
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Reverse engineering the way humans rank textures

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
3
1
1

Citation Types

0
10
0

Year Published

2011
2011
2018
2018

Publication Types

Select...
4
4

Relationship

0
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 9 publications
(10 citation statements)
references
References 24 publications
0
10
0
Order By: Relevance
“…In addition to color, voice-triggered synesthetes often report texture perceptions, for example a voice might be “smooth but granulated” or “[with a] soft center and very slight fuzziness around the outside.” Despite our knowledge of perceptions such as these, no systematic investigation of visual texture perceptions in synesthesia has yet been conducted—which is perhaps not surprising considering that it is not easy to quantify texture or to relate this quantification to perceptual categories (Petrou et al, 2007; Clarke et al, 2011). Eagleman and Goodale (2009) state: “Quantitatively testing these prevalences [of texture concurrents] will be a challenge: it is straightforward to develop a user-friendly color chooser […], but not so with the multidimensional varieties of texture” (Eagleman and Goodale, 2009, 291).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…In addition to color, voice-triggered synesthetes often report texture perceptions, for example a voice might be “smooth but granulated” or “[with a] soft center and very slight fuzziness around the outside.” Despite our knowledge of perceptions such as these, no systematic investigation of visual texture perceptions in synesthesia has yet been conducted—which is perhaps not surprising considering that it is not easy to quantify texture or to relate this quantification to perceptual categories (Petrou et al, 2007; Clarke et al, 2011). Eagleman and Goodale (2009) state: “Quantitatively testing these prevalences [of texture concurrents] will be a challenge: it is straightforward to develop a user-friendly color chooser […], but not so with the multidimensional varieties of texture” (Eagleman and Goodale, 2009, 291).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…However, visual inspection using a dendrograms 1 suggest that the data are reasonable. Furthermore, the perceptual similarity matrix has been extensively used to design intuitive database and browsing environments and allows for statistically significantly faster navigation [6] when compared to environments based on the trace transform [11]. However, the authors recognise that this possibility can by no means be dismissed and what is really required is to significantly increase the numbers of observers in the grouping experiments and we have planned a series of further observer sessions that we will analyse later this year.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…These results were questioned by Heaps & Hande [8] who argued that a dimensional model of texture is inappropriate and that texture similarity judgements are context dependent. Better results have been obtained for computational similarity by Long & Leow [9] and Petrou et al [11] however, these again used the limited Brodatz album which provides a relatively sparse sampling of texture space.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 93%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…The Trace Transform (TT) [5] has characterized and classified successfully and automatically different types of images [6] [7] [8]. This method extracts features associated with the image using several functionals, one after another, to reduce the complexity of the image.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%