1960
DOI: 10.1037/h0048601
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Retroactive and proactive inhibition of verbal learning.

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1

Citation Types

1
51
1

Year Published

1963
1963
2013
2013

Publication Types

Select...
7
3

Relationship

0
10

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 70 publications
(53 citation statements)
references
References 82 publications
1
51
1
Order By: Relevance
“…In proactive interference the associations acquired during Phase 1 interfere with the retrieval (or acquisition) of the target associations trained during Phase 2, and in retroactive interference the associations acquired during Phase 2 interfere with the retrieval of the target associations trained during Phase 1. The concepts of proactive interference and retroactive interference developed primarily within the early studies of verbal behavior, especially those using paired-associate stimuli (for reviews, see Slamecka & Ceraso, 1960;Swenson, 1941). In a typical study of retroactive cue interference in verbal paired-associate learning, human participants were first presented with a list of pairedassociates (e.g., words, nonsense syllables, or trigrams) of the form A-B (e.g., cat-tree), then a second list of the form C-B (e.g., horse-tree).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…In proactive interference the associations acquired during Phase 1 interfere with the retrieval (or acquisition) of the target associations trained during Phase 2, and in retroactive interference the associations acquired during Phase 2 interfere with the retrieval of the target associations trained during Phase 1. The concepts of proactive interference and retroactive interference developed primarily within the early studies of verbal behavior, especially those using paired-associate stimuli (for reviews, see Slamecka & Ceraso, 1960;Swenson, 1941). In a typical study of retroactive cue interference in verbal paired-associate learning, human participants were first presented with a list of pairedassociates (e.g., words, nonsense syllables, or trigrams) of the form A-B (e.g., cat-tree), then a second list of the form C-B (e.g., horse-tree).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…A substantial body of evidence on interference between cues trained apart comes also from the classical studies on retroactive interference in the verbal learning tradition (e.g., Slamecka & Ceraso, 1960;Underwood, 1966). Although researchers in the interference tradition more extensively studied retroactive interference with the A-B, A-C paradigm (i.e., two different outcomes associated to a common cue; e.g., Bäuml, 1996Bäuml, , 1998Chandler, 1993;Chandler & Gargano, 1998), many experiments have shown that interference can also take place when two different cues are associated to a common outcome (i.e., the A-B, C-B paradigm; see, e.g., Abra, 1967;Cheung & Goulet, 1968;Johnston, 1968;Keppel, Bonge, Strand, & Parker, 1971;Schwartz, 1968).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…More specifically, we were concerned with the effects of learning a second solution to a problem on recall of the first solution. The experimental design that is most often used to test this hypothesis is the retroactive design (Deese and Hulse, 1967;Slamecka and Ceraso, 1960), in which an experimental group learns A, then learns B, and is then tested on A, while a control group learns A, then rests (i.e., performs a neutral task), and is finally tested on A. Performance on the A test can be compared for the two groups to provide evidence of transfer.…”
Section: Retroactive Interference Of Similar Methods To Teach Translamentioning
confidence: 99%