2021
DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.659451
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Resuming a Dynamic Task Following Increasingly Long Interruptions: The Role of Working Memory and Reconstruction

Abstract: Studies examining individual differences in interruption recovery have shown that higher working memory capacity (WMC) attenuated the negative impact of interruption length on resumption, at least in static contexts. In continuously evolving (or dynamic) situations, however, working memory may not be as central to the effective resumption of a task, especially in the case of long interruptions. One of the main theories of task interruption suggests that dynamic task resumption could depend on a reconstruction … Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
4
1

Citation Types

0
7
0

Year Published

2021
2021
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
4
1

Relationship

1
4

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 5 publications
(7 citation statements)
references
References 50 publications
0
7
0
Order By: Relevance
“…The impact of interruptions on search resumption was influenced by the nature of the interrupting event. Longer ( Labonté and Vachon, 2021 ) and more difficult ( Brazzolotto and Michael, 2020 ) interruptions significantly delayed the resumption time compared to shorter or simpler ones. Similarly, interruptions involving emotionally charged stimuli extended resumption time more than neutral interruptions ( Brazzolotto and Michael, 2021 ).…”
Section: Resultsmentioning
confidence: 95%
See 3 more Smart Citations
“…The impact of interruptions on search resumption was influenced by the nature of the interrupting event. Longer ( Labonté and Vachon, 2021 ) and more difficult ( Brazzolotto and Michael, 2020 ) interruptions significantly delayed the resumption time compared to shorter or simpler ones. Similarly, interruptions involving emotionally charged stimuli extended resumption time more than neutral interruptions ( Brazzolotto and Michael, 2021 ).…”
Section: Resultsmentioning
confidence: 95%
“…A majority of research in artificial environments (10/18) was conducted with the task of identifying a specific letter among distractors ( Lleras et al, 2005 , 2007 ; Beck et al, 2006 ; Van Zoest et al, 2007 ; Jungé et al, 2009 ; Lleras and Enns, 2009 ; Thomas and Lleras, 2009 ; Höfler et al, 2011 ; Godwin et al, 2013 ; Mereu et al, 2014 ). Furthermore, five of the studies asked participants to identify a target shape ( Olds et al, 2000a , b , c , 2001 ; Olds and Punambolam, 2002 ) while one study each explored finding a picture ( Alonso et al, 2021 ), categorizing numbers ( Ratwani and Trafton, 2008 ) and tracking multiple objects ( Labonté and Vachon, 2021 ). In contrast to the more homogenous distribution of search tasks in studies conducted in artificial environments, natural environment search tasks exhibited greater heterogeneity.…”
Section: Resultsmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 2 more Smart Citations
“…A widespread example of a complex span task is the operation span (or OSpan), in which participants have to recall letters in serial order while making arithmetical judgments (e.g., Unsworth et al, 2005 ). For many complex tasks, a higher WMC is associated with better performance: for instance, reading complicated texts online ( Schurer et al, 2020 ), recalling visual material in the presence of irrelevant sound ( Hughes et al, 2013 ), resuming interrupted dynamic tasks ( Labonté & Vachon, 2021 ), executing complex attacks in volleyball ( Bisagno & Morra, 2018 ), or piloting planes ( Cak et al, 2020 ). However, high-WMC individuals do not outperform their low-WMC counterparts in every context.…”
Section: Wmcmentioning
confidence: 99%