2022
DOI: 10.1080/0361073x.2022.2033592
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Responsible Remembering and Forgetting in Younger and Older Adults

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
4

Citation Types

1
22
0

Year Published

2022
2022
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
6

Relationship

4
2

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 18 publications
(23 citation statements)
references
References 71 publications
1
22
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Overall, these results are informative for understanding potential boundary conditions under which young and older adults prioritize learned information for later recall. Future work should systematically study how differences between motivated remembering and forgetting (e.g., Adcock et al, 2006; Bowen et al, 2020; Mather & Schoeke, 2011; Miendlarzewska et al, 2016; Murphy & Castel, 2022b, 2022c; Spaniol et al, 2014) interact with the time during the task where young and older adult participants are made aware of the relevant reward-oriented goal state.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Overall, these results are informative for understanding potential boundary conditions under which young and older adults prioritize learned information for later recall. Future work should systematically study how differences between motivated remembering and forgetting (e.g., Adcock et al, 2006; Bowen et al, 2020; Mather & Schoeke, 2011; Miendlarzewska et al, 2016; Murphy & Castel, 2022b, 2022c; Spaniol et al, 2014) interact with the time during the task where young and older adult participants are made aware of the relevant reward-oriented goal state.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly. between motivated remembering and forgetting (e.g., Adcock et al, 2006;Bowen et al, 2020;Mather & Schoeke, 2011;Miendlarzewska et al, 2016;Murphy & Castel, 2022b, 2022cSpaniol et al, 2014) interact with the time during the task where young and older adult participants are made aware of the relevant reward-oriented goal state. Also evident from the present findings is older adults' maintained ability to prioritize high-value information at least as effectively as young adults.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…In everyday life, we are continuously presented with more information than we can remember and must selectively focus on the most important information with consequences for forgetting to maximize memory utility (e.g., Ariel et al, 2009;Castel et al, 2012;Madan, 2017;McGillivray & Castel, 2011;Murphy & Castel, 2020, 2021a, 2021b, 2022bMurphy et al, 2022). However, most previous work examining how learners use value to guide memory processes has utilized procedures whereby participants are presented with a list of information to remember, are tested on that information, and then are presented with the next list of information followed by another test, with each test only examining memory for the just-studied list of information.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…In the context of direct forgetting (intentional or goal-directed forgetting of certain information), after studying a list of information, both younger and older adults can forget information if they are instructed to forget it (e.g., Biss et al, 2013; Bowen et al, 2020; Murphy & Castel, 2022b; Sahakyan et al, 2008; Sego et al, 2006; Zacks et al, 1996; and see Titz & Verhaeghen, 2010, for a meta-analysis), suggesting that some forms of strategic forgetting are intact in older age (but may be impaired in older old adults over the age of 75; see Aslan & Bäuml, 2013). This mechanism of strategic or adaptive forgetting reduces interference in some situations, although it is unclear how this may differentially influence memory for lower and higher value information (but see Bowen et al, 2020, for a demonstration of how financial reward anticipation can boost younger and older adults’ memory for both to-be-remembered and to-be-forgotten items in a directed forgetting task, indicating that high values can strengthen memory without enhancing strategic control over memory).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Prior work has demonstrated that both younger and older adults used importance to guide the encoding and retrieval of information (Murphy & Castel, 2022a), but this had yet to be tested in an associative memory task (whereby older adults show profound deficits, e.g., Arenberg & Robertson-Tchabo, 1977; Naveh-Benjamin, 2000; Service & Craik, 1993). Additionally, prior work has shown little or no benefit of value in some standard associative learning tasks (Ariel et al, 2015).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%