1987
DOI: 10.1901/jeab.1987.48-383
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Response‐reinforcer Relations and the Maintenance of Behavior

Abstract: The effects on pigeons' key pecking of unsignaled delays of reinforcement and response-independent reinforcement were compared after either variable-interval or differential-reinforcement-of-low-rate baseline schedules. One 30-min session arranging delayed reinforcement and one 30-min session arranging response-independent reinforcement were conducted daily, 6 hr apart. A within-subject yoked-control procedure equated reinforcer frequency and distribution across the two sessions. Response rates usually were re… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
3

Citation Types

1
30
0
4

Year Published

1992
1992
2022
2022

Publication Types

Select...
7

Relationship

4
3

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 33 publications
(35 citation statements)
references
References 10 publications
1
30
0
4
Order By: Relevance
“…Future research should determine whether alternative reinforcers with higher value paid immediately suppress drug choice to a greater degree under this arrangement. Alternative reinforcer magnitude and the immediacy of reinforcement both impact reinforcing efficacy (Gleeson and Lattal, 1987; Nader and Woolverton, 1991). In addition, this study did not include an inactive placebo control condition.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Future research should determine whether alternative reinforcers with higher value paid immediately suppress drug choice to a greater degree under this arrangement. Alternative reinforcer magnitude and the immediacy of reinforcement both impact reinforcing efficacy (Gleeson and Lattal, 1987; Nader and Woolverton, 1991). In addition, this study did not include an inactive placebo control condition.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…It may be that our procedures were more similar to those used in the nonhuman and child literature. Specifically, many of the studies with nonhumans and studies with older children involved the repeated presentation of the delayed reinforcement contingency within subjects and across sessions (R. Baer et al, 1984;Ferster & Hammer, 1965;Fowler & Baer, 1981;Gleeson & Lattal, 1987;Sizemore & Lattal, 1977). Although Millar (1972), Millar and Watson (1979), and Ramey and Ourth (1981) used within-subject designs, they did not provide repeated exposure across daily sessions, nor was a criterion of stability achieved prior to changing experimental conditions.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…When the temporal interval between a response and a consequence is lengthened, the subsequent association may be weaker than associations formed with more contiguous events. The association between delayed reinforcement and responding has been investigated in several species, including rats (Grice, 1948;Wolfe, 1934), pigeons (Dews, 1960;Ferster, 1953;Gleeson & Lattal, 1987;Sizemore & Lattal, 1977;Williams, 1976), and monkeys (Ferster & Hammer, 1965), in addition to human infants (Millar, 1972;Millar & Watson, 1979;Ramey & Ourth, 1971) and children (R. Baer, Williams, Osnes, & Stokes, 1984;Fowler & Baer, 1981 …”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…So, whether rats emitted more behavior under one condition or the other depends upon the dimension measured. It may be of interest to test if parallel findings are obtained with longer delays, particularly those which have failed to convincingly increase behavior (see Gleason & Lattal, ; Snycerski, Laraway, Byrne, & Poling, ). For example, precedent suggests that imposing an unsignaled resetting delay of 8 s or longer would reduce lever pressing compared to behavior contacting an FR 1 (Wilkenfield et al, ).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%