1976
DOI: 10.3758/bf03214057
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Resistance to extinction of a learned taste aversion varies with time of conditioning

Abstract: Sixteen rats were maintained out of doors in cages with natural light, temperature, and social stimulation for 3 months. Subsequently, by pairing the taste of sucrose with IP injections of LiCI, the rats were conditioned to avoid sucrose. Each of four groups of rats received the CS-US pairing at a different time of day. Times of conditioning were 6 a.m., 12 a.m., 6 p.m. and 12 p.m. EST. A twobottle preference test between 4% sucrose solution and tap water was initiated 24-h after conditioning. Daily measuremen… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1

Citation Types

0
10
0

Year Published

1977
1977
2005
2005

Publication Types

Select...
7
1

Relationship

0
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 16 publications
(10 citation statements)
references
References 33 publications
(28 reference statements)
0
10
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Extinction rates reported in the literature vary considerably as a result of the extent of food or water deprivation, time of testing, and method of CS delivery (e.g., Abelson, Pierrel-Sorrentino, & Blough, 1977;Fouquet, Oberling, & Sandner, 2001;Nolan et al, 1997;Ternes, 1976;J. Yamamoto, Fresquet, & Sandner, 2002).…”
Section: Extinctionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Extinction rates reported in the literature vary considerably as a result of the extent of food or water deprivation, time of testing, and method of CS delivery (e.g., Abelson, Pierrel-Sorrentino, & Blough, 1977;Fouquet, Oberling, & Sandner, 2001;Nolan et al, 1997;Ternes, 1976;J. Yamamoto, Fresquet, & Sandner, 2002).…”
Section: Extinctionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…In one study, performance efficiency, but not acquisition, was affected by time of day of testing (Ghiselli and Patton, 1976); in another, initial performance was equivalent among groups, but resistance to extinction varied dramatically with time of training (Ternes, 1976). Electroconvulsive shock (ECS) given to mice immediately after training disrupted memory equally well at two times of day, but ECS given 3 min after training disrupted memory only in mice tested during the D phase and not in those tested in L (Stephens, McGaugh, and Alpern, 1967).…”
Section: Learning and Memorymentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The performance of laboratory animals on a learning task varies with the phase of an LD cycle at which they are tested; such variations have been reported for conditioned suppression (Stroebel, 1967;Evans and Patton, 1970); avoidance learning (Davies, Navaratnam, and Redfern, 1973;Ghiselli and Patton, 1976;Gordon and Scheving, 1968); maze learning (Hostetter, 1966;Stavnes, 1972); and taste-aversion learning (Rusak and Zucker, 1974;Ternes, 1976). Daily rhythms might influence learning and performance in several ways; (1) variations in arousal, motivation (e.g., hunger), or sensory thresholds during acquisition and/or testing could modify the salience of discriminative cues as well as the value of rewards or punishments; (2) the ability to consolidate and store acquired information might vary with time of training; and (3) the ability to retain and recall stored information might vary with time of testing.…”
Section: Learning and Memorymentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Circadian rhythms, the fluctuations of several biological functions over a 24-h period, have been found to correlate with a variety of behavioral measures. Research with infrahumans has revealed a number of significant relationships between circadian rhythms and processes such as learning, memory, and motivation (e.g., Stephens & McGaugh, 1968;Stroebel, 1967;Ternes, 1976). Of particular interest are recent studies which suggest that both adult and child psychotics exhibit abnormal circadian rhythm functions (Hill, Wagner, Shedlarski, & Sears, in press;Morgan & Cheadle, 1976).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%