1969
DOI: 10.3758/bf03329134
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Resistance to extinction in children as a function of N-length and number of different N-lengths

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

0
6
0

Year Published

1971
1971
1996
1996

Publication Types

Select...
5

Relationship

0
5

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 8 publications
(6 citation statements)
references
References 7 publications
(7 reference statements)
0
6
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Most investigations of the PREE manipulated the percentage of reinforced responses or trials during acquisition and demonstrated that resistance was inversely related to these percentages. However, results of studies with both humans and nonhumans, conducted primarily by Capaldi and colleagues, suggest that resistance to extinction is determined by other factors related to reinforcement schedules, such as the number of consecutive nonreinforced trials preceding a reinforced trial (N length), the number of different N lengths, and the number of each N length (e.g., Capaldi, 1964;Halpern & Poon, 1971;Litchfield & Duerfeldt, 1969;Meyers & Capaldi, 1970). In addition, the PREE is more likely to occur when PRF is combined with other variables, including lengthy acquisition training (Uhl & Young, 1967), large reinforcement magnitudes (Amsel, Hug, & Surridge, 1968), delayed reinforcement (L. Peterson, 1956), and massed acquisition trials (Sheffield, 1949).…”
Section: Basic (Nonclinical) Researchmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Most investigations of the PREE manipulated the percentage of reinforced responses or trials during acquisition and demonstrated that resistance was inversely related to these percentages. However, results of studies with both humans and nonhumans, conducted primarily by Capaldi and colleagues, suggest that resistance to extinction is determined by other factors related to reinforcement schedules, such as the number of consecutive nonreinforced trials preceding a reinforced trial (N length), the number of different N lengths, and the number of each N length (e.g., Capaldi, 1964;Halpern & Poon, 1971;Litchfield & Duerfeldt, 1969;Meyers & Capaldi, 1970). In addition, the PREE is more likely to occur when PRF is combined with other variables, including lengthy acquisition training (Uhl & Young, 1967), large reinforcement magnitudes (Amsel, Hug, & Surridge, 1968), delayed reinforcement (L. Peterson, 1956), and massed acquisition trials (Sheffield, 1949).…”
Section: Basic (Nonclinical) Researchmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…problem-solving conditions (e.g., Halpern & Poon, 1971;Lamberth, 1971;Lamberth & Dyck, 1972;Litchfield & Duerfeldt, 1969;Meyers & Capaldi, 1970), as well as analogue clinical conditions (e.g., Jones, Nation, & Massad, 1977;Koegel & Rincover, 1977;Nation & Massad, 1978).…”
Section: Smentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Preliminary evidence from these investigations and at least four others (Halpern & Poon, 1971;Lamberth, 1971;Litchfield & Duerfeldt, 1969;Meyers & Capaldi, 1970) indicate that researchers interested in human conditioning should consider these more specific operations when studying an analog to instrumental reward conditioning. The effects of sequence of reward in acquisition are, according to Capaldi, a fundamental difference between sequential and nonsequential theory.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 89%
“…on each clock for the remaining trials. This procedure was necessary because the results of pilot work and other investigations (Lamberth, Gouaux, & Davis, in press;Litchfield & Duerfeldt, 1969) indicated that if 5 is prompted to pull the lever by E after the start of the experiment, he pulls for many trials at a steady rate regardless of the reinforcement contingencies.…”
Section: Experiments Imentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation