1999
DOI: 10.1080/07370024.1999.9667267
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Representing Cognitive Activity in Complex Tasks

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

0
15
0

Year Published

2000
2000
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
7

Relationship

1
6

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 17 publications
(16 citation statements)
references
References 0 publications
0
15
0
Order By: Relevance
“…interface designers [1,10,13]. The rows of the notation correspond to successive attentional fixations upon a processing topic (shown in a black frame), with the superordinate grouping of that topic, its predicate and its constituent structures detailed.…”
Section: Transition Path Diagramsmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…interface designers [1,10,13]. The rows of the notation correspond to successive attentional fixations upon a processing topic (shown in a black frame), with the superordinate grouping of that topic, its predicate and its constituent structures detailed.…”
Section: Transition Path Diagramsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Our approach to modelling structure and cognition is constructed within Barnard's Interacting Cognitive Subsystems (ICS) framework [1]. ICS allows the construction of approximate models of cognition which, without needing to model the exact nature of the transformations of information involved in any particular situation, can provide parameterised descriptions of the complexity of the cognitive activity that is required.…”
Section: Structures In Mental Representationsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Barnard [1991] and Barnard and May [1999] have argued for families of theoretical models that address different aspects of users' cognition and interactive system designs with a means of exchanging knowledge between different theories and models. Modelers from cognitive, AI, and software engineering backgrounds can contribute solutions to design problems from different perspectives using design rationale to summarize design issues and modeling recommendations [Bellotti 1993]; however, this study did not show how knowledge could be exchanged between models from separate academic traditions, even though some progress has been made on linking cognitive and software engineering models [Harrison and Barnard 1993].…”
Section: Theories and Cognitive Modelsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Unfortunately, design rationale has little to offer in organizing the semantics of knowledge exchange, so more rigorous definitions of HCI issues and arguments are required to create a lingua franca for designers and researchers. Barnard and May [1999] provide a partial answer in Transition [speech track] "... Next, the protein pyrimidine complex absorbs light from the visible range …" The design question is, which components will the user attend to and comprehend from this multimodal presentation? Propositions have to encode the objects, captions, commands, speech segments, and timing, and this changes for each frame in an animated sequence.…”
Section: Theories and Cognitive Modelsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Drawing upon Newell's [1990] proposals, in the modeling of a psychological system of interactors "segments" can be represented by very short term activities lasting a few hundred milliseconds. A "phase" might be represented by periods of up to 10 seconds [Barnard and May 1999]. Changes in psychological capability brought about by knowledge acquisition and learning would naturally encompass far longer time scales.…”
Section: Macrotheory and Interactionmentioning
confidence: 99%