1991
DOI: 10.1037/0022-3514.61.2.181
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Representativeness and conjoint probability.

Abstract: People commonly violate a basic rule of probability, judging a conjunction of events to be more probable than at least 1 of its component events. Many manifestations of this conjunction fallacy have been ascribed to people's reliance on the representativeness heuristic for judging probability. Some conjunction fallacies, however, have been ascribed to the incorrect rules people use to combine probabilities. In 2 experiments, representativeness was pitted against probability combination to determine the contrib… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

4
136
0
2

Year Published

1995
1995
2022
2022

Publication Types

Select...
5
1
1

Relationship

0
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 87 publications
(142 citation statements)
references
References 27 publications
4
136
0
2
Order By: Relevance
“…Gavanski and Roskos-Ewoldsen (1991) did not test (41) directly, although it is a testable condition, but rather they tested a consequence of (41), namely, that the average number of conjunction errors should be about the same for related and unrelated conjunctions. Gavanski and Roskos-Ewoldsen (1991) found no significant differences in the frequency of conjunction errors produced by related and unrelated conjunctions. The statistics reported in their study do not permit one to evaluate the power of their tests, but the fact that the null result was found in experiments with samples of 180 and 153 subjects, respectively, suggests that statistical power was sufficient to detect differences of moderate size.…”
Section: ____________________________________________________________mentioning
confidence: 61%
See 2 more Smart Citations
“…Gavanski and Roskos-Ewoldsen (1991) did not test (41) directly, although it is a testable condition, but rather they tested a consequence of (41), namely, that the average number of conjunction errors should be about the same for related and unrelated conjunctions. Gavanski and Roskos-Ewoldsen (1991) found no significant differences in the frequency of conjunction errors produced by related and unrelated conjunctions. The statistics reported in their study do not permit one to evaluate the power of their tests, but the fact that the null result was found in experiments with samples of 180 and 153 subjects, respectively, suggests that statistical power was sufficient to detect differences of moderate size.…”
Section: ____________________________________________________________mentioning
confidence: 61%
“…Perhaps the similarity of the results for related and unrelated events is merely a coincidence resulting from a particular choice of propositions. Gavanski and Roskos-Ewoldsen (1991) provided evidence against this objection, as well as a sharper test of the generality of probability combination models. They selected propositions A 1 , A 2 , B 1 , and B 2 such that A 1 and A 2 pertained to the same issue, and B 1 and B 2 pertained to some other issue.…”
Section: ____________________________________________________________mentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…as regras estatísticas e leis de probabilidade (e.g., Gavanski & Roskos-Ewoldsen, 1991;Stolarz-Fantino, Fantino & Kulik, 1996;Tversky & Kahneman, 1983;Wolford, Taylor & Beck, 1990). Os julgamentos intuitivos da probabilidade de um evento composto são baseados em heurísticas da representatividade, as quais negligenciam considerações outras que não aquelas relacionadas com a similaridade entre um exemplo e um modelo.…”
Section: C Rodrigues E J Abreu-rodriguesunclassified
“…Nos dois casos, a identificação da regra que controlou o julgamento da probabilidade do composto é baseada na comparação entre as probabilidades do composto e de seus constituintes. Controle pela soma das probabilidades dos constituintes é inferido quando a probabilidade estimada do composto é maior do que a probabilidade de ambos os eventos constituintes (e.g., Fantino & Savastano, 1996;Gavanski & Roskos-Ewoldsen, 1991;Rodrigues, 2005). Por outro lado, controle pela média das probabilidades dos constituintes é sugerido quando a probabilidade estimada do composto é maior do que a probabilidade do evento constituinte menos provável e menor do que aquela do evento constituinte mais provável (e.g., Fantino, Kulik, Stolarz-Fantino & Wright, 1997;Gavanski & Roskos-Ewoldsen, 1991;Shanteau, 1975;Zizzo, StolarzFantino, Wen & Fantino, 2000).…”
Section: Regras De Probabilidadeunclassified