2018
DOI: 10.1177/0165025418820689
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Representational momentum in displacement tasks: Relative object weight matters in toddlers’ search behavior

Abstract: Various developmental studies have demonstrated that implied object weight is a key variable in children’s interpretations of motion, such as predicting the objects’ speeds. An additional bias in predictions of object motion is representational momentum (RM), where objects are anticipated to be found in a location farther along in the direction of motion from where they actually are. The present study aimed to evaluate when children begin to be sensitive to relative weight in a RM-related search task. Toddlers… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1

Citation Types

2
1
0

Year Published

2020
2020
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
3
1

Relationship

1
3

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 4 publications
(4 citation statements)
references
References 29 publications
(48 reference statements)
2
1
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Arithmetic operations are represented as moving of attentional focus along mental number line from the first operand position and proportionally to the second operand, but ‘overshooting’ the expected outcome position. This resembles the phenomenon of 'representational momentum' observed in adults and young children (Freyd & Finke, 1984; Hast, 2019; Hubbard, Matzenbacher, & Davis, 1999) whereby the expected final position of the moving (or apparently moving) object is typically overestimated in the direction of movement. Per analogiam , addition moves attention along the number line toward larger numbers and the 'momentum' of this movement produces overestimation bias, whereas subtraction directs attention toward smaller numbers, and the 'momentum' produces underestimation bias (cf.…”
Section: Introductionsupporting
confidence: 63%
“…Arithmetic operations are represented as moving of attentional focus along mental number line from the first operand position and proportionally to the second operand, but ‘overshooting’ the expected outcome position. This resembles the phenomenon of 'representational momentum' observed in adults and young children (Freyd & Finke, 1984; Hast, 2019; Hubbard, Matzenbacher, & Davis, 1999) whereby the expected final position of the moving (or apparently moving) object is typically overestimated in the direction of movement. Per analogiam , addition moves attention along the number line toward larger numbers and the 'momentum' of this movement produces overestimation bias, whereas subtraction directs attention toward smaller numbers, and the 'momentum' produces underestimation bias (cf.…”
Section: Introductionsupporting
confidence: 63%
“…Allen's (2015) study on toddlers illustrates this by showing that 3-yearolds appear to possess a slightly less archetypal definition of "animal" than 5-year-olds do. This also coincides with the suggestion that the explicit formation of scientific misconceptions begins to show first signs before 3 years of age, and that the development of language plays a key role in the generation of such conceptions (Hast, 2018(Hast, , 2019Mandler, 2004). Beyond this early stage, a range of studies in different linguistic contexts have demonstrated there is subsequently no single definition for the "animal" category in later development.…”
Section: The Development Of "Animal"supporting
confidence: 84%
“…This seems to be in line with the suggestion that more restrictive archetypal scientific definitions begin to emerge at around 3 to 4 years of age (cf. Hast, 2018Hast, , 2019Mandler, 2004). The older children's significantly improved performance can be associated with educational experience as their testing occurred after having been taught about animals in school, as per the national syllabus in Singapore (MOE, 2014).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Children exhibit a wide range of ideas regarding the functioning of the world (Hast, 2020a;Hast & Howe, 2012). These ideas emerge early in their developmental trajectory, typically surfacing by the age of 3 years (see e.g., Hast, 2018Hast, , 2019Povinelli et al, 2012). Notably, it remains a well-established observation that changing such conceptions continues to pose challenges, even for educators themselves (Hast, 2017).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%