2021
DOI: 10.31234/osf.io/rm52c
|View full text |Cite
Preprint
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Representational exchange in human social learning: Balancing efficiency and flexibility

Abstract: What makes human social learning so powerful? While past accounts have sometimes prioritized finding the single capacity that makes the largest difference, our social learning abilities span a wide spectrum of capacities from the high-fidelity imitation of behaviors to inferring and learning from hidden mental states. Here, we propose that the power of human social learning lies not within a single capacity, but in our ability to flexibly arbitrate between different computations and to integrate their outputs.… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

0
12
0

Year Published

2022
2022
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
5
3

Relationship

1
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 17 publications
(15 citation statements)
references
References 57 publications
(82 reference statements)
0
12
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Specifically, teachers chose examples that would be most informative to a learner who interprets them literally-that is, one who rules out the options that are contradicted by the examples provided and is indifferent between the rest. Although coordinated use of recursive mentalizing would have accelerated learning slightly in our task (e.g., in the toy example in Figure 2), it is plausible that teachers may have eschewed this approach due to its computational demands (Hawkins et al, 2021;Wu et al, 2021), or to ensure that their examples are understood by a broad audience of learners of varying degrees of sophistication (Frank & Liu, 2018).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Specifically, teachers chose examples that would be most informative to a learner who interprets them literally-that is, one who rules out the options that are contradicted by the examples provided and is indifferent between the rest. Although coordinated use of recursive mentalizing would have accelerated learning slightly in our task (e.g., in the toy example in Figure 2), it is plausible that teachers may have eschewed this approach due to its computational demands (Hawkins et al, 2021;Wu et al, 2021), or to ensure that their examples are understood by a broad audience of learners of varying degrees of sophistication (Frank & Liu, 2018).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Much like a thrifty shopper or an efficient long-distance runner, adaptive organisms should not only maximize rewards, but also account for the cognitive costs of different strategies. While resource-rational adaptations have been widely studied in the context of individual decision making (Kool, Gershman, & Cushman, 2018;Shenhav et al, 2017), we propose that a similar trade-off exists in social learning (Wu, Vélez, & Cushman, 2022).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 92%
“…The representational exchange framework of social learning (Wu, V élez, & Cushman, 2022) describes how different social learning mechanisms form a hierarchy, trading off computational costs against flexibility and compositionality (Fig. 1).…”
Section: Social Learning Hierarchymentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Whereas action copying operates over motor programs, value inference estimates representations of instrumental value (i.e., goals), requiring an extra layer of inference and incurring putatively greater computational costs (Wu et al, 2022). However, it may offer better generalization to new situations.…”
Section: World Modelmentioning
confidence: 99%