2007
DOI: 10.1287/mksc.1060.0241
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Representation and Inference of Lexicographic Preference Models and Their Variants

Abstract: The authors propose two variants of lexicographic preference rules. They obtain the necessary and sufficient conditions under which a linear utility function represents a standard lexicographic rule, and each of the proposed variants, over a set of discrete attributes. They then: (i) characterize the measurement properties of the parameters in the representations; (ii) propose a nonmetric procedure for inferring each lexicographic rule from pairwise comparisons of multiattribute alternatives; (iii) describe a … Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

2
83
0

Year Published

2008
2008
2020
2020

Publication Types

Select...
7
2

Relationship

0
9

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 97 publications
(85 citation statements)
references
References 48 publications
2
83
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Even just a single exposure can lead people to consider buying novel brands (Coates, Butler, & Berry, 2004, 2006. The hypothesis that people rely on the recognition heuristic is also consistent with theories of consideration-set identification that assume noncompensatory heuristics (Kohli & Jedidi, 2007). In fact, the recognition heuristic was initially proposed as the first step in take-the-best (Gigerenzer & Goldstein, 1996), a noncompensatory heuristic for two-alternative decisions.…”
Section: Elimination By Recognitionsupporting
confidence: 63%
“…Even just a single exposure can lead people to consider buying novel brands (Coates, Butler, & Berry, 2004, 2006. The hypothesis that people rely on the recognition heuristic is also consistent with theories of consideration-set identification that assume noncompensatory heuristics (Kohli & Jedidi, 2007). In fact, the recognition heuristic was initially proposed as the first step in take-the-best (Gigerenzer & Goldstein, 1996), a noncompensatory heuristic for two-alternative decisions.…”
Section: Elimination By Recognitionsupporting
confidence: 63%
“…When John Hauser, a proponent of conjoint analysis, began to test models of heuristics, he found to his surprise that sequential heuristics predict consumer choices well (Hauser et al 2009). Examples are decisions between computers (Kohli & Jedidi 2007) and smartphones (Yee et al 2007). In particular, heuristics are important early in the decision process to form a consideration set, which consists of eliminating most products from further consideration.…”
Section: Figurementioning
confidence: 99%
“…Generally speaking, linear compensatory choice models do not address simplifying choice heuristics such as truncation and level focus that can result in an abrupt change in choice probability. Yet, noncompensatory simple heuristics are often more or at least equally accurate in predicting new data compared to linear models that are criticized for over-fitting the data [67,89,103]. While the linear utility model has been the mainstay in conjoint research, Bayesian methods, including data augmentation, can easily accommodate nonlinear models and can deal with irregularities in the likelihood surface [6].…”
Section: A2 Compensatory Versus Noncompensatory Processesmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…While the linear utility model has been the mainstay in conjoint research, Bayesian methods, including data augmentation, can easily accommodate nonlinear models and can deal with irregularities in the likelihood surface [6]. Recently, Kohli and Jedidi [103] and Yee et al [190] propose dynamic programming methods (using greedy algorithm) to estimate lexicographic preference structures. Noncompensatory processes are particularly relevant in the context of consideration sets, an issue typically ignored by the traditional conjoint research (e.g., [67,91]).…”
Section: A2 Compensatory Versus Noncompensatory Processesmentioning
confidence: 99%