2005
DOI: 10.1191/0265659005ct288oa
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Repair in speech and language therapy interaction: Investigating pragmatic language impairment of children

Abstract: This paper examines the repair skills of three groups of 7 to 11-year old children: 1) children with Pragmatic Language Impairments (the PLIgroup); 2) children with Specific Language Impairments with no pragmatic difficulties (the SLI group); 3) children with normally developing communication skills (the mainstream group). The data comprise one-to-one interactions with a speech and language therapist, where the participants are engaged in a task devised to provide multiple opportunities for the initiation of r… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
4
1

Citation Types

0
28
0

Year Published

2009
2009
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
5
3

Relationship

0
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 26 publications
(28 citation statements)
references
References 19 publications
(11 reference statements)
0
28
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Brinton and Fujiki (1982) report that children with language impairment used fewer requests for clarification in a referential communication task than children with typically developing communication skills. According to Merrison and Merrison (2005) children with pragmatic language impairment did not initiate repairs as frequently as children with language impairment without pragmatic problems and children with typical language development when participating in a referential communication task. Donahue et al (1980) suggest that children with learning disabilities are less likely to initiate repairs in conversation than normal controls.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 84%
See 2 more Smart Citations
“…Brinton and Fujiki (1982) report that children with language impairment used fewer requests for clarification in a referential communication task than children with typically developing communication skills. According to Merrison and Merrison (2005) children with pragmatic language impairment did not initiate repairs as frequently as children with language impairment without pragmatic problems and children with typical language development when participating in a referential communication task. Donahue et al (1980) suggest that children with learning disabilities are less likely to initiate repairs in conversation than normal controls.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 84%
“…The task usually involves different kinds of physical items, or pictures depicting different kinds of items, that vary on certain dimensions (for example, size or colour). Referential communication tasks have been widely used in studying different clinical populations (Brinton and Fujiki 1982, Bishop and Adams 1991, Leinonen and Letts 1997, Reuterskiöld Wagner et al 2001, Merrison and Merrison 2005). Brinton and Fujiki (1982) report that children with language impairment used fewer requests for clarification in a referential communication task than children with typically developing communication skills.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Simoneau et al ., ; Williamson et al ., ) and language‐related impairments (e.g. Merrison & Merrison, ; Dockrell, Lindsay & Connelly, ).…”
Section: Theories Of the Origins Of Languagementioning
confidence: 99%
“…Repair trajectories are particularly delicate in contexts in which language impairments are a central issue (Milroy and Perkins 1992;Merrison and Merrison 2005). In speech therapy interactions, repair sequences create opportunities to focus on instructional issues (Ridley et al 2002;Radford 2008) as they may challenge the linguistic and interactional competence of the patient (Wilkinson 2007), which may trouble the progression of the interaction.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%