1995
DOI: 10.3758/bf03199936
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Reinforcement accounts for transitive inference performance

Abstract: Transitive inference is the ability, given that A >B andB > C, to infer that A > C. Pigeons, rats, chimpanzees, squirrel monkeys, and humans as young as 4 years have all been shown capable of this. In this paper, simple associative learning models are explored as accounts of nonverbal transitive inference performance. A Bush-Mosteller-based model can account for transitive inference under limited conditions. A Rescorla-Wagner-based model can account for transitive inference under all conditions in the literatu… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

3
131
0

Year Published

1998
1998
2017
2017

Publication Types

Select...
7
1

Relationship

0
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 109 publications
(134 citation statements)
references
References 32 publications
3
131
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Though we do not claim that the algorithm is psychologically realistic, it provides a functional model that can deal with partial orderings of elements coded by high-dimensional feature vectors. The function performed by this model is consistent with empirical evidence that both animals and humans can learn simple orderings from a partial set of ordered pairs (Merritt & Terrace, 2011;Trabasso & Riley, 1975;Woocher, Glass, & Holyoak, 1978;Wynne, 1995). Moreover, its output (feature weights) can readily be translated into empirical priors for learning one-place predicates.…”
Section: Multiple Levels Of Representation For Comparative Relationssupporting
confidence: 74%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Though we do not claim that the algorithm is psychologically realistic, it provides a functional model that can deal with partial orderings of elements coded by high-dimensional feature vectors. The function performed by this model is consistent with empirical evidence that both animals and humans can learn simple orderings from a partial set of ordered pairs (Merritt & Terrace, 2011;Trabasso & Riley, 1975;Woocher, Glass, & Holyoak, 1978;Wynne, 1995). Moreover, its output (feature weights) can readily be translated into empirical priors for learning one-place predicates.…”
Section: Multiple Levels Of Representation For Comparative Relationssupporting
confidence: 74%
“…However, there is ample evidence that many types of animals can learn simple orderings from a partial set of pairs. For both animals (Merritt & Terrace, 2011;Wynne, 1995) and humans (Trabasso & Riley, 1975;Woocher et al, 1978), orderings are typically learned ''from the ends in'', with the extreme or ''landmark'' objects being acquired prior to those that lie closer to the middle of a continuum. In the present simulations, we trained RankSVM with ordered pairs that mainly involved the half dozen animals with the highest or lowest values on the relevant continuum.…”
Section: Deriving Magnitudes From Unstructured Feature Vectorsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Direct encoding can be based on a physical attribute such as size but is most commonly interpreted in terms of the associative strength that each stimulus accumulates as a consequence of its history of reward and nonreward (Couvillon & Bitterman, 1992). Strength is assumed to build up both directly through rewarded responses to the stimuli themselves (Wynne, 1995(Wynne, , 1997 and in some models, indirectly, through transfer of value from other simultaneously presented and rewarded stimuli (von Fersen, Wynne, Delius, & Staddon, 1991;Zentall & Clement, 2001;Zentall & Sherburne, 1998). Direct representation is not purely a function of associative strength, however.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Critically, the A and B stimuli elicited higher happiness ratings and greater right prefrontal alpha activation than the D and E stimuli. Given that neither the A nor the E stimuli constituted "end anchors" of the series, the pre versus post differences are unlikely to have been driven by associative value transfer processes (e.g., Wynne, 1995). The high happiness ratings and FAAs reported for Stimulus C during baseline and post measurements suggest a relation between FAA and valence, motivation and/or familiarity (cf.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Second, when comparing the pre and post valences measured for the face stimuli, the valences for some stimuli (particularly Stimulus A) were shown to be enhanced in the post condition, countering common knowledge regarding generalization gradients (e.g., Cofer & Foley, 1942;Hermans, Baeyens, & Vervliet, 2013). On balance, the "enhancement" may have been driven at least partially by the "end-anchor" effect (Wynne, 1995), which simply means that the stimulus whose selection was always reinforced (Stimulus A) and the stimulus whose selection was never reinforced (Stimulus E) evoked differentially ratings/FAAs due to their differential reinforcement histories.…”
Section: Assessing Tof Through Contextually Controlled Transitive Infmentioning
confidence: 99%