2020
DOI: 10.1177/0963721420917688
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Regret and Decision-Making: A Developmental Perspective

Abstract: Regret is a common emotion that has important links with decision-making in adults. Recent research suggests that the ability to experience regret emerges relatively late in development. By around 6 years, most children will experience regret, but the likelihood of experiencing this emotion increases across childhood and into adolescence. The developmental emergence of regret seems to affect children’s decision-making: Children who experience regret about a choice are more likely to make a better choice next t… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

1
8
0

Year Published

2021
2021
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
7
3

Relationship

1
9

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 33 publications
(12 citation statements)
references
References 29 publications
1
8
0
Order By: Relevance
“…The ability to anticipate emotions (e.g. regret over choices made for oneself) has been found to develop later than their expression [ 85 , 86 ]. In addition, children's choice to reject advantageous inequity is, in part, strategically motivated by a desire to appear concerned with fairness towards observers—especially peers who would be disadvantaged by inequity ([ 87 , 88 ]; see also [ 89 ]).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The ability to anticipate emotions (e.g. regret over choices made for oneself) has been found to develop later than their expression [ 85 , 86 ]. In addition, children's choice to reject advantageous inequity is, in part, strategically motivated by a desire to appear concerned with fairness towards observers—especially peers who would be disadvantaged by inequity ([ 87 , 88 ]; see also [ 89 ]).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…This seems unlikely given evidence that children can by age 5 anticipate future events (Atance, 2015) and accurately report that “today” precedes “next week,” which precedes “next year” (Tillman et al, 2017). At the same time, children’s temporal cognition (e.g., Atance, 2015; McCormack, 2015; McCormack & Hoerl, 2017; Tillman et al, 2017) and experiences of related emotions (e.g., “regret”; McCormack et al, 2020) continue to develop over early childhood, and these developments may underlie shifts in children’s thinking about emotional injury. An interesting related question is whether duration of harm may be more important to some children’s reasoning than others’.…”
Section: Methodsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…It also enables retrospective learning about counterfactual actions and consequences, as when we imagine what might have happened had we pursued an alternative course of action in the past [73,74]. Such vicarious learning can be especially potent when the imagined consequences are emotionally charged [75], as in the experience of regret when comparing a sub-optimal past choice to a superior but counterfactual past choice [76]. Here, Gautam et al .…”
Section: Ultimate Questions: Functions and Phylogenymentioning
confidence: 99%