1959
DOI: 10.1016/0001-6918(59)90011-3
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Réflexions sur le roˆle du langage dans l'analyse des organisations perceptives

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1

Citation Types

1
13
0

Year Published

1961
1961
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
6
2

Relationship

0
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 12 publications
(15 citation statements)
references
References 4 publications
1
13
0
Order By: Relevance
“…The task of the observer in a perception experiment is nevertheless to a large extent a cognitive one-to describe or judge what he perceives (Michotte, 1959). Therefore it must be made very clear to the observer that his cognitive efforts should be directed towards communicating, in the most faithful way, his percepts-not towards elaborating and supplementing them.…”
Section: Sensory Psychophysicsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The task of the observer in a perception experiment is nevertheless to a large extent a cognitive one-to describe or judge what he perceives (Michotte, 1959). Therefore it must be made very clear to the observer that his cognitive efforts should be directed towards communicating, in the most faithful way, his percepts-not towards elaborating and supplementing them.…”
Section: Sensory Psychophysicsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Research on amodal completion ( Kanizsa, 1985 ; Michotte, Thinès, & Crabbé, 1964 ; van Lier & Gerbino, 2015 ), however, indicates that our mental representations of the occluded parts of a visual scene often have surprisingly much in common with ordinary visual representations. First, as illustrated in Figure 1 , our experience of the hidden parts of an object often involves a curious and compelling sense of “perceptual presence” ( Leddington, 2009 ) or “amodal presence” ( Michotte et al., 1964 ). Second, as also illustrated by the same demonstration, the processes underlying our experience of the occluded parts often seem to operate in an automatic and subjectively effortless way.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Third, our experience of the occluded parts of objects often has functional influences on other aspects of perceptual processing such as the perception of motion ( Holcombe, 2003 ; Kim, Feldman, & Singh, 2012 ; McDermott, Weiss, & Adelson, 2001 ; Scherzer & Ekroll, 2009 , 2012 ; Shimojo & Nakayama, 1990 ), object recognition ( Johnson & Olshausen, 2005 ), transparency perception ( Anderson & Schmid, 2012 ), and even the felt size of the observer’s own body parts ( Ekroll, Sayim, Van der Hallen, & Wagemans, 2016 ). Fourth, our experience of the hidden parts of objects is often impervious to conscious knowledge or reasoning ( Gerbino & Zabai, 2003 ; Kanizsa, 1985 ; Michotte et al., 1964 ; Nielsen, 2008 ). For instance, a semispherical shell viewed from the convex side will continue to look like a complete ball even when the observer knows that it is just a hollow shell ( Ekroll, Sayim, & Wagemans, 2013 ; Ekroll et al., 2016 ).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…As it refers to occluded objects it would seem weird to categorize it as a visual phenomenon, but on the other hand, it is very similar to visual phenomena in other regards. In particular, as emphasized by several researchers ( Ekroll, Mertens, et al., 2018 ; Ekroll et al., 2016 ; Gerbino & Zabai, 2003 ; Kanizsa, 1985 ; Michotte et al., 1964 ), illusions based on amodal completion tend to persist in spite of conflicting conscious knowledge, in the same way as visual illusions at large. That is, they are cognitively impenetrable ( Firestone & Scholl, 2016 ; Pylyshyn, 1999 ).…”
mentioning
confidence: 87%
“…The illusion of absence is reminiscent of the well-known phenomenon of amodal completion ( Figure 1C and D ) because it also refers to a curiously compelling experience of occluded regions in a visual scene. Amodal completion refers to experiences of complete objects partially hidden behind an occluder, which are particularly compelling although only a few fragments of the object are directly visible ( Kanizsa, 1985 ; Michotte et al., 1964 ; Scherzer & Faul, 2019 ; Thielen et al., 2019 ; Van Lier & Gerbino, 2015 ). Figure 1D , for instance, evokes a very compelling experience of a single long finger, rather than the two separate fingers, which are aligned behind the cylinder ( Figure 1C ).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%