1995
DOI: 10.1017/s0305000900009983
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Reference states and reversals: undoing actions with verbs

Abstract: The purpose of these studies is to characterize children's conception of reversal and its relation to a reference state. A reversal is the move from one state to some prior state of affairs. For example, shoes that have been TIED can be UNTIED, parcels WRAPPED then UNWRAPPED, and dishes COVERED then UNCOVERED. The present studies were designed to find out how children (aged 1;0 to 5;0) describe reversals of action that restore objects to a prior, less constrained, state. In English, the prefix un- offers the m… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

1
18
0

Year Published

1998
1998
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
6
2
1

Relationship

0
9

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 33 publications
(19 citation statements)
references
References 14 publications
1
18
0
Order By: Relevance
“…The model succeeded in capturing the basic developmental stages for reversives reported by Bowerman (1982) and Clark, Carpenter, and Deutsch (1995). In particular, the model was able to produce overgeneralization errors such as "*unbreak" or "*disbend".…”
Section: The Organization Of Semantic Fieldsmentioning
confidence: 71%
“…The model succeeded in capturing the basic developmental stages for reversives reported by Bowerman (1982) and Clark, Carpenter, and Deutsch (1995). In particular, the model was able to produce overgeneralization errors such as "*unbreak" or "*disbend".…”
Section: The Organization Of Semantic Fieldsmentioning
confidence: 71%
“…He found that English-speaking four-year-olds use a lexical strategy, which usually involves an antonym ('The ship is small'), as often as they use syntactic negation strategies ('The ship is not large'), whereas Japanese four-year-olds used the antonym strategy only 25% of the time. Clark, Carpenter & Deutsch (1995) examined how young children describe reversals of action. In one of their experiments, two-, three-and four-year-old English speakers gave commands to reverse an action that had been described using a nonce verb plus a 'positive' (up, on, in) or 'negative' (down, off, out) verb particle.…”
Section: Volume 28 Issuementioning
confidence: 99%
“…The literature suggests that children are indeed able to use these to constrain verb meanings (e.g., Fisher, Hall, & Gleitman, 1994;Gleitman, 1990, for the evidence for the former source; Choi & Bowerman, 1991, for the latter). However, it is not clear how strongly these constraints are applied, or to what extent children rely on contextual and/or pragmatic information (Clark, 1997;Clark, Carpenter, & Deutsch, 1995;Tomasello, 1997), or to what extent fast-mapping is possible and successful for verbs. We would like to conclude by noting that we need to investigate lexical acquisition in a broad perspective, studying the acquisition of a wide range of lexical classes, including nouns, verbs, prepositions and classifiers, and to consider how different lexical structures influence patterns of word learning.…”
Section: Implications For the Theory Of Early Word Learningmentioning
confidence: 99%