2001
DOI: 10.3758/bf03194419
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Reference frames and haptic perception of orientation: Body and head tilt effects on the oblique effect

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1

Citation Types

8
69
4
1

Year Published

2003
2003
2017
2017

Publication Types

Select...
6
1
1

Relationship

1
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 84 publications
(82 citation statements)
references
References 39 publications
(45 reference statements)
8
69
4
1
Order By: Relevance
“…Studies of kinesthetic perception have shown the existence of biases in the estimation of distances in tactile-kinesthetic tasks such as blindfolded triangle completion (Klatzky, 1999). The geometry of perceived haptic space can be distorted by temporal factors (Dupin, Hayward, & Wexler, 2015;Lederman, Klatzky, Collins, & Wardell, 1987;Yusoh, Nomura, Sakamoto, & Iwabu, 2012), movement speed (Kazunori, Akinori, Daisuke, & Ito, 2006;Viviani, Baud-Bovy, & Redolfi, 1997;Wapner, Weinberg, Glick, & Rand, 1967;Whitsel et al, 1986), memory (Chieffi, Conson, & Carlomagno, 2004;Gentaz & Hatwell, 1999;Millar & Al-Attar, 2004) and the configuration of the body, notably the hands (Kaas & Mier, 2006) or the head and body (Luyat, Gentaz, Corte, & Guerraz, 2001). Biases in the perception of orientation have been observed when participants have to orient a bar in order to make it parallel in threedimensional space to another bar (Kappers, 1999;Kappers & Koenderink, 1999).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Studies of kinesthetic perception have shown the existence of biases in the estimation of distances in tactile-kinesthetic tasks such as blindfolded triangle completion (Klatzky, 1999). The geometry of perceived haptic space can be distorted by temporal factors (Dupin, Hayward, & Wexler, 2015;Lederman, Klatzky, Collins, & Wardell, 1987;Yusoh, Nomura, Sakamoto, & Iwabu, 2012), movement speed (Kazunori, Akinori, Daisuke, & Ito, 2006;Viviani, Baud-Bovy, & Redolfi, 1997;Wapner, Weinberg, Glick, & Rand, 1967;Whitsel et al, 1986), memory (Chieffi, Conson, & Carlomagno, 2004;Gentaz & Hatwell, 1999;Millar & Al-Attar, 2004) and the configuration of the body, notably the hands (Kaas & Mier, 2006) or the head and body (Luyat, Gentaz, Corte, & Guerraz, 2001). Biases in the perception of orientation have been observed when participants have to orient a bar in order to make it parallel in threedimensional space to another bar (Kappers, 1999;Kappers & Koenderink, 1999).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…First, orientation discrimination thresholds increase from approximately 0.5° (when presenting the visual stimulus along the retinal vertical) to 1.5-2.0° when the visual stimulus is roll-tilted 30° (Orban et al 1984), while we observed greater variability for the earth-fixed paradigm (~3° in upright position and ~4° in 75° RED). Second, the oblique effect decreases or even disappears when the head is roll-tilted, (Luyat et al 2001;McIntyre et al 2001). …”
Section: For Visually-guided Self-adjustments Extra-retinal Cues Arementioning
confidence: 99%
“…However, trial-to-trial variability of alignment tasks increases independently from the presence / absence of retinal input when the subject is roll-tilted (Tarnutzer et al 2012), so increased variability in our tasks cannot be explained solely on the basis of variability of torsional eye position. At the same time, the oblique effect diminishes when the head is rolltilted (Luyat et al 2001;McIntyre et al 2001), mostly due to increased discrimination thresholds along the principal axes. Therefore, the accuracy by which the roll orientation of a visual stimulus on the retina can be sensed becomes independent from its retinal roll orientation (in relation to the retinal cardinal axes).…”
Section: For Visually-guided Self-adjustments Extra-retinal Cues Arementioning
confidence: 99%
“…Spatial information is always specified with respect to some frame of reference (Howard, 1982) and many studies on the perception of orientations have focused on identifying it (Kappers, 1999(Kappers, , 2003(Kappers, , 2004Luyat et al, 2001;Volcic, van Rheede, Postma, & Kappers, 2008;Volcic et al, 2007). However, the concept of reference frame per se does not explain why or how directional anisotropies occur within the possibly rotated subjective frame of reference since it fails to explain what might cause orientation-specific variations in the accuracy and precision of the responses such as the directional bias toward the diagonal.…”
Section: Limits Of the Extended Ca Modelmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…In fact, the focus in the latter tasks has been on the variable error and, in this context, the term oblique effect usually refers to a decrease in precision at the oblique orientations relative to the cardinal (vertical or horizontal) orientations, which has been observed both in the visual (reviews in Gentaz & Ballaz, 2000;Gentaz & Junker-Tschopp, 2002) and haptic modalities (e.g. Appelle & Countryman, 1986;Gentaz & Hatwell, 1995, 1999Lechelt, Eliuk, & Tanne, 1976;Lechelt & Verenka, 1980;Luyat, Gentaz, Corte, & Guerraz, 2001). Still, it is noteworthy that, in the visual modality, several studies have reported orientation-dependent biases in orientation tasks that don't involve the transfer of orientation information between two different positions (e.g.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%