1981
DOI: 10.1037/0033-2909.89.2.325
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Reevaluation of the literature on the development of transitive inferences.

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1

Citation Types

9
100
0

Year Published

1982
1982
2011
2011

Publication Types

Select...
4
3
1

Relationship

1
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 124 publications
(109 citation statements)
references
References 52 publications
9
100
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Only at age 8 years did children now consistently choose the correct item (A) regardless of whether filler-blocks were used or not (overall three-term performance = 76%). Ameel, Verschueren and Schaeken (2007) confirmed similar levels for 8 year-olds in two separate Transitivity for Height v Speed 9 experiments, but also concluded that deductive-transitive-inference development is still not complete at age 8 (Kallio, 1988;Rabinowitz et al, 1994; (Breslow, 1981). Put another way, training is used to give the participant a head start on the exact problems s/he will face during test trials.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 64%
See 2 more Smart Citations
“…Only at age 8 years did children now consistently choose the correct item (A) regardless of whether filler-blocks were used or not (overall three-term performance = 76%). Ameel, Verschueren and Schaeken (2007) confirmed similar levels for 8 year-olds in two separate Transitivity for Height v Speed 9 experiments, but also concluded that deductive-transitive-inference development is still not complete at age 8 (Kallio, 1988;Rabinowitz et al, 1994; (Breslow, 1981). Put another way, training is used to give the participant a head start on the exact problems s/he will face during test trials.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 64%
“…Regarding transitive reasoning, Kallio (1982) found that in order to succeed on his task within the Extensive-Training-Paradigm, children first demonstrated internalisation of the entire transitive series given by the pair-wise information, constructing an internal mental representation of the linearly graded scale (Breslow, 1981;Halford & Andrews, 2004;Wright, 2001). Note, Transitivity for Height v Speed 23 children generally tended to be able to generalise the series, only if the premises had been given in both directions and were fully-ordered (see also Riley & Trabasso, 1974;Stromer et al, 1993;Siegal, 2003;Wright, 2006b).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…heavy) and its negation (not heavy). Comparisons involving the ' not heavy' end-point or items associated with this anchor through proximity (Breslow, 1981), add to the RT through the operation of negation. The more likely it is that a decision can be made without negation-by using the positive, (e.g.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…In order to avoid such memory deficits, they trained children on the premises of transitive inferences and found that even 4-year-olds could solve the respective problems under these conditions. A number of subsequent researchers also re-ported transitive reasoning in preschool children (see reviews by Breslow, 1981: Halford, 1982Thayer & Collyer, 1978). Although Piaget denied that age was a criterion for his cognitive developmental stages {e.g.. Piaget in Bringuier, 1980), these results were apparently discrepant with his own findings and cast doubt on both his explanations of transitive reasoning and his methods for assessing it.…”
Section: Transitivity In \Bung Childrenmentioning
confidence: 99%