1995
DOI: 10.1207/s15516709cog1901_2
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Reduced Memory Representations for Music

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1

Citation Types

0
23
0

Year Published

2001
2001
2022
2022

Publication Types

Select...
6
1
1

Relationship

0
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 37 publications
(25 citation statements)
references
References 37 publications
0
23
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Levitin and Cook (1996) found the recollection of a song's tempo to be extremely similar to that of the song's actual tempo, suggesting that musical memory is absolute (accurate). Similarly, Large et al (1995) gathered evidence for the reductionist mechanism of memory representations, finding that a piece of attended music is often reduced in substance, such that only its musical gist is retained in memory. Finally, in research examining the relation between age and memory for music, Gaudreau and Peretez (1999) observed similar effects of aging on musical memory to those on verbal auditory memory.…”
Section: Memory For Musicmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Levitin and Cook (1996) found the recollection of a song's tempo to be extremely similar to that of the song's actual tempo, suggesting that musical memory is absolute (accurate). Similarly, Large et al (1995) gathered evidence for the reductionist mechanism of memory representations, finding that a piece of attended music is often reduced in substance, such that only its musical gist is retained in memory. Finally, in research examining the relation between age and memory for music, Gaudreau and Peretez (1999) observed similar effects of aging on musical memory to those on verbal auditory memory.…”
Section: Memory For Musicmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…For example, as early as infancy, listeners can recognize a melody as the same when it is transposed to a different set of frequencies, performed at a different rate or tempo, or performed in a different instrumental timbre or "voice" (Chang & Trehub, 1977;Trehub & Thorpe, 1989). Even music whose pitch or duration content has changed, as in variations on musical themes, permits perceptual constancy (Dowling & Harwood, 1986;Large, Palmer, & Pollack, 1995;Serafine, Glassman & Overbeeke, 1989). Despite the many types of stimulus variability that music displays across performers, rates, timbres, and contexts, music perception is robust and adaptable over a range of physical changes in the acoustic signal.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Large, Palmer, & Pollack, 1995). Normalization refers to the process of converting or transforming physically different tokens into some common representational format that is the ultimate goal of perceptual analysis, and these standardized representations are stored in memory (Pisoni, 1997).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…In the field of musicology, structural points in music have been characterized as nodes in "time-span reductions" (Lerdahl and Jackendoff 1983) or in "event hierarchies" (Bharucha 1984). Empirical research has found that structural tones are important elements in musical improvisation (Large, Palmer, and Pollack 1995) and listening (Bigand and Parncutt 1999).…”
Section: Goal-oriented Behaviormentioning
confidence: 99%