The Psychology of Problem Solving 2003
DOI: 10.1017/cbo9780511615771.002
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Recognizing, Defining, and Representing Problems

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1

Citation Types

3
208
0
8

Year Published

2010
2010
2022
2022

Publication Types

Select...
5
3
1

Relationship

0
9

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 208 publications
(219 citation statements)
references
References 31 publications
3
208
0
8
Order By: Relevance
“…We used diagrams of two domains to ensure that our results were not context-specific. We chose auction and travel as the two domains because we expected them to be moderately familiar to subjects (neither extremely familiar nor extremely unfamiliar), which our pretests subsequently confirmed (average auction domain knowledge = 4.21/7.00; average travel domain knowledge = 3.97/7.00).It was important to choose moderately familiar domains because, although prior domain knowledge was not a construct of interest in our study, it can strongly affect how people interpret diagrams (Pretz et al 2003).For each domain, we selected excerpts from ontologies that were developed and are used in practice. These ontologies are available in formal OWL notation at www.schemaweb.…”
mentioning
confidence: 83%
“…We used diagrams of two domains to ensure that our results were not context-specific. We chose auction and travel as the two domains because we expected them to be moderately familiar to subjects (neither extremely familiar nor extremely unfamiliar), which our pretests subsequently confirmed (average auction domain knowledge = 4.21/7.00; average travel domain knowledge = 3.97/7.00).It was important to choose moderately familiar domains because, although prior domain knowledge was not a construct of interest in our study, it can strongly affect how people interpret diagrams (Pretz et al 2003).For each domain, we selected excerpts from ontologies that were developed and are used in practice. These ontologies are available in formal OWL notation at www.schemaweb.…”
mentioning
confidence: 83%
“…A distinction drawn in the literature is between 'well-defined' tasks like the TOL, and 'ill-defined' problems (Reitman, 1965). In an ill-defined problem, the solver may not initially possess all the necessary information: that is, the goal state, operators or constraints might be ambiguous, or there could be more than one correct solution (Kahney, 1986;Pretz, Naples, & Sternberg, 2003). Ill-defined problems are more likely to be encountered in everyday life than in the laboratory (Dunbar, 1998).…”
Section: Problem Solvingmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Current theories of problem solving mostly reflect the results of research conducted on well-structured problems, while ill-structured problems are ill understood (Pretz, Naples, & Sternberg, 2003). However, more focus on ill-structured problems is necessary.…”
Section: Fostering Transfermentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Another distinguishing feature of ill-structured problem solving is the combination of multi-contextual influences and dynamics of uncertainty (Mirel, 2004). Often, the problem solver has to take different perspectives on a problem before finding one that gives insights into viable solution paths (Pretz et al, 2003). Research points out that different intellectual skills are needed for solving well-structured problems, which rely on applicative or recurrent skills that are highly domain-specific, and ill-structured problems, which rely not only on applicative but also on interpretive or non-recurrent skills that are less domain-specific (Cho & Jonassen, 2002;Hong, Jonassen, & McGee, 2003;Van Merriënboer, 1997).…”
Section: Fostering Transfermentioning
confidence: 99%