2016
DOI: 10.1080/23273798.2016.1154974
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Recognising a zebra from its stripes and the stripes from “zebra”: the role of verbal labels in selecting category relevant information

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
3
1
1

Citation Types

1
14
0

Year Published

2016
2016
2022
2022

Publication Types

Select...
6
1
1

Relationship

4
4

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 12 publications
(15 citation statements)
references
References 52 publications
1
14
0
Order By: Relevance
“…While previous developmental research testing the hypothesis that labels direct attention to commonalities often used basic or dense categories ( Best et al, 2010 , 2011 ), effects of labels appear to be important for learning of sparse categories ( Perry and Lupyan, 2016 ), and it is possible that some of the discrepancies in previously reported research stems from using dense categories that can be learned without supervision. Dense categories typically have a high family resemblance and have many correlated features (e.g., birds typically have beaks, feathers, small bodies, chirp, etc.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 86%
See 2 more Smart Citations
“…While previous developmental research testing the hypothesis that labels direct attention to commonalities often used basic or dense categories ( Best et al, 2010 , 2011 ), effects of labels appear to be important for learning of sparse categories ( Perry and Lupyan, 2016 ), and it is possible that some of the discrepancies in previously reported research stems from using dense categories that can be learned without supervision. Dense categories typically have a high family resemblance and have many correlated features (e.g., birds typically have beaks, feathers, small bodies, chirp, etc.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 86%
“…Moreover, selective attention abilities correlate with categorization of sparse categories. In particular, adults’ performance on a flanker task requiring them to specify the direction of a central target while ignoring irrelevant distractors on either the right or left sides (measure of selective attention) was associated with how quickly participants verified the names of labels denoting sparse categories ( Perry and Lupyan, 2016 ). On a related note, there is evidence with 9- to 15-month-old infants that exposure to linguistic input has a larger effect on learning of sparse or more general categories, whereas, dense/basic categories can often be learned without exposure to labels ( Waxman and Markow, 1995 ; Fulkerson and Haaf, 2003 ).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Verbal interference requires people to dual-task which has some undesired properties and so more recently we have turned to noninvasive brain stimulation as a way to subtly up- or down-regulate linguistic processing (Perry & Lupyan, 2013). These studies have provided additional support that implicit verbal labelling leads to more categorical representations (Perry & Lupyan, 2014, 2017b).…”
Section: The Influence Of Verbal Labels On the Representation Of Meaningmentioning
confidence: 77%
“…Most measures focus on the consistency of participant's responding. For instance, one classic measure computes the name agreement by determining what percentage of participants give the modal response (Brandimonte et al, 1992;Brodeur et al, 2010;Perry & Lupyan, 2016). A second measure is entropy of the naming responses (Brodeur et al, 2010;Snodgrass & Vanderwart, 1980), defined as:…”
Section: Nameability Definedmentioning
confidence: 99%