2018
DOI: 10.1177/0301006618781088
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Reasoning About Visibility in Mirrors: A Comparison Between a Human Observer and a Camera

Abstract: Human observers make errors when predicting what is visible in a mirror. This is true for perception with real mirrors as well as for reasoning about mirrors shown in diagrams. We created an illustration of a room, a top-down view, with a mirror on a wall and objects (nails) on the opposite wall. The task was to select which nails were visible in the mirror from a given position (viewpoint). To study the importance of the social nature of the viewpoint, we divided the sample ( N = 108) in two groups. One group… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

0
16
0

Year Published

2019
2019
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
8

Relationship

2
6

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 10 publications
(16 citation statements)
references
References 28 publications
0
16
0
Order By: Relevance
“…This is not to say that many authors within this field do not refer to and examine the computation of the relative positions of humans and objects. Indeed, there does seem to be something qualitatively different about representing a scene from a different position when that position is occupied by another human as opposed to an inanimate object (e.g., Bertamini & Soranzo, 2018;Tversky & Hard, 2009). We do, however, suggest, or perhaps remind authors, that it is not possible to take the perspective, the percept, of another person.…”
Section: The Perspective-taking Homunculusmentioning
confidence: 64%
See 2 more Smart Citations
“…This is not to say that many authors within this field do not refer to and examine the computation of the relative positions of humans and objects. Indeed, there does seem to be something qualitatively different about representing a scene from a different position when that position is occupied by another human as opposed to an inanimate object (e.g., Bertamini & Soranzo, 2018;Tversky & Hard, 2009). We do, however, suggest, or perhaps remind authors, that it is not possible to take the perspective, the percept, of another person.…”
Section: The Perspective-taking Homunculusmentioning
confidence: 64%
“…Result showed that the number was reported from the direction/viewpoint of the chair to the same degree as when an agent sat in the chair. As noted previously, however, this is not to say that there is nothing special about having an agent in a display when observers are asked to make judgements that rely on the coding of spatial relationships (e.g., Becchio, Del Giudice, Dal Monte, Latini-Corazzini, & Pia, 2013;Bertamini & Soranzo, 2018;Tversky & Hard, 2009). Thus, a nonhuman reference point may not induce effects to the same degree as a human agent.…”
Section: The Agent As a Directional Cuementioning
confidence: 94%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Therefore, it will be important going forward for researchers to be specific about which type of perspective-taking is under examination. Lastly, evidence from the current issue suggests that certain effects might depend on the cognitive demand of the experimental task [19,34,49], which indicates that social factors are involved when the task is cognitively demanding, while they may not be necessary in other cases.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 84%
“…When a human agent is present in a display, responses on a variety of tasks are facilitated (e.g., [ 59 ]). It is not yet clear what exactly the agent provides that a non-agent does not.…”
Section: Representation Rather Than Functionmentioning
confidence: 99%