2000
DOI: 10.1111/1467-7687.00129
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Reasoning about collisions involving inert objects in 7.5‐month‐old infants

Abstract: The present research asked whether 7.5-month-old infants realize that an object cannot displace another object without contacting it. The infants in Experiment 1 were assigned to a contact or a no-contact condition. The infants in the nocontact condition saw static familiarization displays in which a tall, thin barrier stood across the bottom of a ramp; a cylinder rested against the left side of the barrier and a wheeled toy bug against its right side. The infants in the contact condition saw similar displays … Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1

Citation Types

5
54
1

Year Published

2002
2002
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
4
2
1
1

Relationship

1
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 80 publications
(65 citation statements)
references
References 47 publications
5
54
1
Order By: Relevance
“…This study establishes a convergence between looking time measures (used here) and the action measures used in closely matched previous work [6]. This convergence may help validate sensitivity to contact causality as an index of causal understanding in infancy research [16,26,27,29,30].…”
Section: Resultssupporting
confidence: 80%
See 2 more Smart Citations
“…This study establishes a convergence between looking time measures (used here) and the action measures used in closely matched previous work [6]. This convergence may help validate sensitivity to contact causality as an index of causal understanding in infancy research [16,26,27,29,30].…”
Section: Resultssupporting
confidence: 80%
“…Nonetheless, prior research suggests that infants' expectations about spatial contact in the domain of physical outcomes vary depending on whether an outcome does or does not occur. In events that adults typically recognize as physical causal interactions, research has shown that infants (1) expect outcomes to occur on contact and (2) expect outcomes not to occur at a distance [16,[26][27][28][29][30]. For example, 7.5-month-old infants have been shown to expect an object to move upon contact from another object, and not to move if an object stops short of contacting it [27].…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…For example, in the case of occlusion events, infants must learn to predict whether an object will be fully or only partly hidden when behind an occluder, and also how soon an object that moves behind an occluder will emerge from behind it; in the case of containment events, infants must learn to predict whether an object can be lowered inside a container, and also how much of an object lowered inside a container will protrude above it; and in the case of collision events, infants must learn to predict whether an object is likely to move when hit, and also how far it is likely to move (e.g. Arterberry, 1997;Aguiar & Baillargeon, 1999;Hespos & Baillargeon, 2001B;Kotovsky & Baillargeon, 2000;Oakes & Cohen, 1995;Sitskoorn & Smitsman, 1995;Spelke, Kestenbaum, Simons, & Wein, 1995a;Wang, Baillargeon, & Brueckner, 2004;Wang, Kaufman, & Baillargeon, 2003;Wilcox & Schweinle, 2003).…”
Section: The Incremental-knowledge Accountmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Attention to movement type (e.g., flexible versus rigid) signals classes of objects that are animate versus inanimate and is critical in determining causality (Golinkoff, Harding, Carlson-Luden, & Sexton, 1984;Kotovsky & Baillargeon, 2000;Mandler, 2004;Cohen & Oakes, 1993;Poulin-Dubois, Lepage, & Ferland, 1996;Rakison, 2003;Wang, Kaufman, & Baillargeon, 2003). Interestingly, while we know a great deal about infants' attention to movement, their understanding of movement has not been studied in its own right.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%