2008
DOI: 10.1080/14622200802163084
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Real-time craving and mood assessments before and after smoking

Abstract: This study explored some quandaries concerning craving and mood as motivators to smoke. Craving and negative mood have long been associated with day-to-day smoking as two of the primary motivational forces behind the maintenance of the behavior, as well as significant barriers in smokers’ attempts to quit. Craving remains a clinically relevant phenomenon, with most smokers describing craving as a troublesome problem when quitting. Smokers’ self-reports of negative mood, as an antecedent for smoking, are so rob… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

6
41
1

Year Published

2009
2009
2022
2022

Publication Types

Select...
9
1

Relationship

0
10

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 61 publications
(49 citation statements)
references
References 19 publications
(28 reference statements)
6
41
1
Order By: Relevance
“…This process sets up positive or negative reinforcement loops respectively, by reinforcing the associative memories between these affective states and smoking (see Figure 1) (Baker et al, 2004;Bevins & Palmatier, 2004;Brandon & Baker, 1991;Carmody, Vieten, & Astin, 2007;Carter et al, 2008;Carter & Tiffany, 2001;Cook et al, 2004;Hall et al, 1993;Hyman, 2007;Rose & Levin, 1991;Warburton & Mancuso, 1998). This associative learning process may then lead to increased motivational salience of future cues (in which both positive and negative cues become more motivationally relevant) (Gross, Jarvik, & Rosenblatt, 1993;Laviolette & van der Kooy, 2004;Olausson, Jentsch, & Taylor, 2004;Robinson & Berridge, 2003;Robinson & Berridge, 1993;Robinson & Berridge, 2008;Waters et al, 2003), resulting in what, building on the work of Baker, Curtin and others (Baker et al, 2004;Curtin et al, 2006), for convenience we term the "addictive loop."…”
Section: Text Boxmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…This process sets up positive or negative reinforcement loops respectively, by reinforcing the associative memories between these affective states and smoking (see Figure 1) (Baker et al, 2004;Bevins & Palmatier, 2004;Brandon & Baker, 1991;Carmody, Vieten, & Astin, 2007;Carter et al, 2008;Carter & Tiffany, 2001;Cook et al, 2004;Hall et al, 1993;Hyman, 2007;Rose & Levin, 1991;Warburton & Mancuso, 1998). This associative learning process may then lead to increased motivational salience of future cues (in which both positive and negative cues become more motivationally relevant) (Gross, Jarvik, & Rosenblatt, 1993;Laviolette & van der Kooy, 2004;Olausson, Jentsch, & Taylor, 2004;Robinson & Berridge, 2003;Robinson & Berridge, 1993;Robinson & Berridge, 2008;Waters et al, 2003), resulting in what, building on the work of Baker, Curtin and others (Baker et al, 2004;Curtin et al, 2006), for convenience we term the "addictive loop."…”
Section: Text Boxmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Cravings are one of the main reasons why people fail to abstain from smoking, alcohol consumption and drug use (Carter et al, 2008). Even in people who have quit substance use, they can often trigger a relapse (Shiffman et al, 1997).…”
Section: Implicit Approach-avoidance Associations For Craved Food Cuesmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Indeed, a number of naturalistic studies have shown that craving is associated with smoking and relapse ( Bagot, Heishman, & Moolchan, 2007 ;Carter et al, 2008 ;Shiffman et al, 2002 ). Not surprisingly, self-report of craving is perhaps the most studied of cue responses.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%