1992
DOI: 10.1016/0042-6989(92)90032-e
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Reading without saccadic eye movements

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
3
2

Citation Types

4
121
1

Year Published

1999
1999
2013
2013

Publication Types

Select...
5
3

Relationship

0
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 168 publications
(131 citation statements)
references
References 18 publications
4
121
1
Order By: Relevance
“…Similarly, although our results do not speak to effects of explicit manipulation of visual attention (Treue and Maunsell, 1996;Connor et al, 1997;Chelazzi et al, 1998;Reynolds et al, 1999), our data are highly relevant to conditions without such pretrial cuing, and robust recognition in clutter is still observed under such conditions (Potter, 1976;Intraub, 1980;Rubin and Turano, 1992). Critically, we have shown that the observed average effect in IT cannot be explained by simple attentional shifts but instead behaves as if it were a primarily feedforward property of the ventral visual stream.…”
Section: Generality Limitations and Implicationscontrasting
confidence: 63%
See 2 more Smart Citations
“…Similarly, although our results do not speak to effects of explicit manipulation of visual attention (Treue and Maunsell, 1996;Connor et al, 1997;Chelazzi et al, 1998;Reynolds et al, 1999), our data are highly relevant to conditions without such pretrial cuing, and robust recognition in clutter is still observed under such conditions (Potter, 1976;Intraub, 1980;Rubin and Turano, 1992). Critically, we have shown that the observed average effect in IT cannot be explained by simple attentional shifts but instead behaves as if it were a primarily feedforward property of the ventral visual stream.…”
Section: Generality Limitations and Implicationscontrasting
confidence: 63%
“…For one, neuronal responses were assessed using a relatively rapid stimulus presentation rate. However, because our presentation rate was similar to that produced by freeviewing monkeys (Motter and Belky, 1998a,b;DiCarlo and Maunsell, 2000;Sheinberg and Logothetis, 2001), gives robust object selectivity in IT (Keysers et al, 2001), and is clearly within the abilities of human recognition (Potter, 1976;Intraub, 1980;Rubin and Turano, 1992), our data provide reasonable estimates of the neuronal responses that underlie fast recognition in clutter. Furthermore, we also found the same average effect for more standard presentation conditions (first stimulus) and previous studies have reported a similar effect in areas V2 and V4 with longer presentation times (Reynolds et al, 1999).…”
Section: Generality Limitations and Implicationssupporting
confidence: 60%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Using digital compression, speech can be accelerated up to ϳ40% of its original duration and remain largely comprehensible (Chodorow, 1979;Mehler et al, 1993;Dupoux and Green, 1997;Pallier et al, 1998;Sebastian-Galles et al, 2000). Furthermore, during reading, expert readers typically attain 250 -300 wpm, and reading speed can be doubled or tripled by removing the need for eye movements (Rubin and Turano, 1992).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The primate visual system has solved this problem: primates robustly and effortlessly discriminate among visual objects over the wide range of images that each object produces during natural vision (Potter, 1976;Intraub, 1980;Rubin and Turano, 1992;Logothetis and Sheinberg, 1996;Thorpe et al, 1996;Edelman, 1999;Rousselet et al, 2004). What neuronal architecture and computations create such a selective and tolerant representation of visual objects?…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%