2017
DOI: 10.1037/xhp0000301
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Reaching into response selection: Stimulus and response similarity influence central operations.

Abstract: iv ABSTRACT This dissertation examines the impact of stimulus and response similarity on response selection. Traditional models of response selection invoke a central processor that operates like a look-up table by matching the perceptually classified stimulus (e.g., green square) to the specified response (e.g., right button press). The look-up property of response selection affords the system the ability to map any stimulus onto any response, even if that stimulus-response has never been paired before. Under… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1

Citation Types

0
1
0

Year Published

2019
2019
2021
2021

Publication Types

Select...
2
1

Relationship

0
3

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 3 publications
(1 citation statement)
references
References 92 publications
(174 reference statements)
0
1
0
Order By: Relevance
“…This paradigm has been used for a range of high-level cognitive tasks, such as social categorization (Freeman et al, 2008, 2013; Freeman & Ambady 2011; Cloutier et al, 2014), processing of grammatical aspect (Anderson et al, 2013), vowel discrimination (Farmer et al, 2009), cognitive flexibility (Dshemuchadse et al, 2015), intertemporal decision-making and delay discounting (Dshemuchadse et al, 2013; Scherbaum et al, 2013, 2016), multitasking (Scherbaum et al, 2015), stimulus-response compatibility (Flumini et al, 2014), lexical decision (Barca & Pezzulo, 2012), and response selection (Wifall et al, 2017). The vast majority of mouse-tracking studies employed the standard two-choice paradigm (Hehman et al, 2015), although some variants have been explored, mostly in a similar methodological frame (e.g., Anderson et al, 2013; Cloutier et al, 2014; Farmer, Anderson, & Spivey, 2007; Farmer, Anderson, & Spivey, 2007; Scherbaum et al, 2013, 2017; Koop & Johnson, 2011).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…This paradigm has been used for a range of high-level cognitive tasks, such as social categorization (Freeman et al, 2008, 2013; Freeman & Ambady 2011; Cloutier et al, 2014), processing of grammatical aspect (Anderson et al, 2013), vowel discrimination (Farmer et al, 2009), cognitive flexibility (Dshemuchadse et al, 2015), intertemporal decision-making and delay discounting (Dshemuchadse et al, 2013; Scherbaum et al, 2013, 2016), multitasking (Scherbaum et al, 2015), stimulus-response compatibility (Flumini et al, 2014), lexical decision (Barca & Pezzulo, 2012), and response selection (Wifall et al, 2017). The vast majority of mouse-tracking studies employed the standard two-choice paradigm (Hehman et al, 2015), although some variants have been explored, mostly in a similar methodological frame (e.g., Anderson et al, 2013; Cloutier et al, 2014; Farmer, Anderson, & Spivey, 2007; Farmer, Anderson, & Spivey, 2007; Scherbaum et al, 2013, 2017; Koop & Johnson, 2011).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%