2019
DOI: 10.1002/tea.21559
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Rational engagement buffers the effect of conservatism on one's reported relevance of the theory of evolution

Abstract: Previous research has noted one's knowledge of and attitudes toward the theory of evolution is negatively predicted by one's political ideology, specifically how conservative the individual identifies, and positively predicted by one's level rational thinking. The present research expands on this past research by examining the roles of political conservatism and rational thinking in predicting one's reported relevance of evolutionary theory in a multi‐study design. In Study 1, a sample of undergraduate college… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1

Citation Types

0
2
0
1

Year Published

2020
2020
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
3
1
1

Relationship

0
5

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 5 publications
(3 citation statements)
references
References 82 publications
0
2
0
1
Order By: Relevance
“…Beyond the pandemic context, people who are more analytic tend to be more skeptical of so-called complementary and alternative medicine (which is often less evidence-based; e.g., using essential oils or homeopathy) (Bowes & Tasimi, 2022;Browne et al, 2015;Budžak & Branković, 2022;Pennycook, Cheyne, et al, 2015. In general, people who are more reflective tend to hold more pro-scientific beliefs across many domains such as biology (e.g., evolution, sex versus gender) (Gervais, 2015;Pennycook, Bago, et al, 2022;Short et al, 2019), neuroscience (e.g., avoiding neuromyths) (van Elk, 2019), astronomy (e.g., big bang) (Pennycook, Bago, et al, 2022), and the social sciences (e.g., psychological misconceptions) (Cho, 2021;Pennycook, Bago, et al, 2022). Indeed, cognitive reflection among school children is predictive of having a stronger conceptual understanding of science (Young & Shtulman, 2020) and people who are more analytic have stronger basic science knowledge (Pennycook, Bago, et al, 2022).…”
Section: Pseudoscience and Alternative Medicinementioning
confidence: 99%
“…Beyond the pandemic context, people who are more analytic tend to be more skeptical of so-called complementary and alternative medicine (which is often less evidence-based; e.g., using essential oils or homeopathy) (Bowes & Tasimi, 2022;Browne et al, 2015;Budžak & Branković, 2022;Pennycook, Cheyne, et al, 2015. In general, people who are more reflective tend to hold more pro-scientific beliefs across many domains such as biology (e.g., evolution, sex versus gender) (Gervais, 2015;Pennycook, Bago, et al, 2022;Short et al, 2019), neuroscience (e.g., avoiding neuromyths) (van Elk, 2019), astronomy (e.g., big bang) (Pennycook, Bago, et al, 2022), and the social sciences (e.g., psychological misconceptions) (Cho, 2021;Pennycook, Bago, et al, 2022). Indeed, cognitive reflection among school children is predictive of having a stronger conceptual understanding of science (Young & Shtulman, 2020) and people who are more analytic have stronger basic science knowledge (Pennycook, Bago, et al, 2022).…”
Section: Pseudoscience and Alternative Medicinementioning
confidence: 99%
“…For instance, Mizrahi (2015) discussed some examples of confirmation bias from the history of science. Rather, it means that science instruction should emphasize the differences between deliberate thoughts and intuitive thoughts as students learn about methods of reasoning (Short et al 2019).…”
Section: Correcting Misbeliefs?mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Recommendations were presented for developing research directions for the curriculum and methodology of teaching the field of science. Manahanm, 2006;Marteel, 2014;Nehring, 2019;Short et al, 2019;Timmer et al, 2018;Skonchal, 2011;Esichaikul et al, 2011;Trybula et al,2009; .) (Georgia et al, 2019;Anderje et al, 2019;Ana et al, 2019;Bachtold et al, 2019;Matuk et al, 2019;Fiedler et al, 2019;Maja et al, 2019;Höft et al, 2019;leuchter & Naber, 2019;Gillespie & Rouse, 2019;Ryoo & Bedell, 2019;Ines, 2019;Elena et al, 2019;Baumflak et al, 2019;She et al, 2019;Curry et al, 2020;Vallera & Bodzin, 2020;Carigetal et al, 2019;Acar, 2019;Sondergeld et al, 2020;Redmond & Cutke, 2020;Lane et al, 2019;Hawley & Sinatra, 2019;Marco etal, 2020) • ‫مع‬ ‫مستخدمه‬ ‫املقارن‬ ‫الوصفي‬ ‫املنهج‬ ‫على‬ ‫اعتمدت‬ ‫اسات‬ ‫در‬ ‫امل‬ ANNOVA ‫القياس‬ ‫موضع‬ ‫املتغير‬ ‫في‬ ‫املتعددة‬ ‫املجموعات‬ ‫بين‬ ‫املتعددة‬ ‫للمقارنات‬ ، ‫ومن‬ ‫اسات:‬ ‫الدر‬ ‫تلك‬ ‫أمثلة‬ Bachtuld et al, 2019;Al-balushi & Martine et al, 2019;Cuzzolino et al, 2019;Plotz, 2019;Eames et al, 2020;Yang et al, 2020;Wikie et al, 2020;Adam, 2019;…”
unclassified