1989
DOI: 10.1037/0021-9010.74.3.433
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Quantitative meanings of verbal probability expressions.

Abstract: The meanings of 18 verbal probability expressions were studied in 3 ways: (a) frequency distributions of what single number best represented each expression; (b) word-to-number acceptability functions from what range of numbers from 0% to 100% best represented each expression; and (c) number-to-word acceptability functions from which expressions were appropriate for multiples of 5% from 5% to 95%. These results agreed highly with others and were highly consistent across methods. Expressions incorporating the s… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

2
83
0

Year Published

1990
1990
2017
2017

Publication Types

Select...
7
2

Relationship

1
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 162 publications
(87 citation statements)
references
References 7 publications
2
83
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Several studies of physicians show similar large interindividual differences in the interpretation of verbal or descriptive terms, even among experienced physicians. 1,2,19,24 We used a rank-ordered list to help decrease interindividual variability and allow better discrimination among terms, compared with random-order lists. 13,14 Even then, when ordered according to assigned percentages (Table 2), only the 1st and last 4 terms remained in their original positions, and the central 10 terms were nearly indistinguishable statistically, similar to the results we obtained for pathologists.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Several studies of physicians show similar large interindividual differences in the interpretation of verbal or descriptive terms, even among experienced physicians. 1,2,19,24 We used a rank-ordered list to help decrease interindividual variability and allow better discrimination among terms, compared with random-order lists. 13,14 Even then, when ordered according to assigned percentages (Table 2), only the 1st and last 4 terms remained in their original positions, and the central 10 terms were nearly indistinguishable statistically, similar to the results we obtained for pathologists.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The CMAH uses the terms 'unlikely,' 'possible,' 'likely,' 'very likely' and 'almost certain' on an ordinal scale to express the likelihood of avalanche(s). Although many studies of quantified verbal probability expressions have identified consistent probability ranges for these terms (e.g., Kent 1964;Reagan et al 1989;Mosteller and Youtz 1990;Mastrandea et al 2010), the scale dependence of probability values and the scale independence of this likelihood terminology rule out associating probability values for this multi-scale approach. When a single slope that is possible to trigger is treated in isolation, it might be considered an unacceptably high risk.…”
Section: Likelihood Of Avalanche(s)mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Although it does not appear in the IPCC-recommended probability lexicon, "possible" is often used to convey uncertainty in the IPCC reports (Intergovernmental Panel on Climate Change, 2014). "Possible" means, on average, a probability range between 35 and 60%, (Budescu & Wallsten, 1985;Juanchich, Sirota, & Butler, 2012;Reagan, Mosteller, & Youtz, 1989). It should therefore be used for outcomes that have a similar frequency of 35-60%.…”
Section: How Likely Are Extreme Outcomes?mentioning
confidence: 99%