2017
DOI: 10.1111/ele.12777
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Quantifying predator dependence in the functional response of generalist predators

Abstract: A long-standing debate concerns how functional responses are best described. Theory suggests that ratio dependence is consistent with many food web patterns left unexplained by the simplest prey-dependent models. However, for logistical reasons, ratio dependence and predator dependence more generally have seen infrequent empirical evaluation and then only so in specialist predators, which are rare in nature. Here we develop an approach to simultaneously estimate the prey-specific attack rates and predator-spec… Show more

Help me understand this report
View preprint versions

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
3
1
1

Citation Types

0
35
0

Year Published

2018
2018
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
6

Relationship

3
3

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 47 publications
(37 citation statements)
references
References 53 publications
0
35
0
Order By: Relevance
“…There are some indications that laboratory experiments may grossly overestimate field‐based feeding rates, although explicit tests are still scarce (Aljetlawi, Sparrevik, & Leonardsson, ; Wilhelm, Schindler, & McNaught, ). In situ functional response experiments are difficult to carry out and, consequently, they are rare (e.g., Jost, Devulder, Vucetich, Peterson, & Arditi, ; Barrios‐O'Neill, Dick, Ricciardi, MacIsaac, & Emmerson, ; Novak, Wolf, Coblentz, & Shepard, ), so data are often combined from a range of locations with the assumption of spatial and temporal consistency (Angerbjorn, Tannerfeldt, & Erlinge, ). Comparative field and laboratory studies are needed to test consistency across contexts (laboratory vs. field) or time (within vs. between years), but the few studies done on this topic are still confounded with the spatial scale of the research (O'Neil, ; Wang & Ferro, ; Xia, Rabbinge, & Werf, ).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…There are some indications that laboratory experiments may grossly overestimate field‐based feeding rates, although explicit tests are still scarce (Aljetlawi, Sparrevik, & Leonardsson, ; Wilhelm, Schindler, & McNaught, ). In situ functional response experiments are difficult to carry out and, consequently, they are rare (e.g., Jost, Devulder, Vucetich, Peterson, & Arditi, ; Barrios‐O'Neill, Dick, Ricciardi, MacIsaac, & Emmerson, ; Novak, Wolf, Coblentz, & Shepard, ), so data are often combined from a range of locations with the assumption of spatial and temporal consistency (Angerbjorn, Tannerfeldt, & Erlinge, ). Comparative field and laboratory studies are needed to test consistency across contexts (laboratory vs. field) or time (within vs. between years), but the few studies done on this topic are still confounded with the spatial scale of the research (O'Neil, ; Wang & Ferro, ; Xia, Rabbinge, & Werf, ).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…This observation has fueled debate over whether and how functional responses should incorporate predator density, but few studies have assessed its importance in the field (Novak et al. ). Our results provide some support for the presence of predator interference in our study system.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…For instance, manipulations that extend the range of predator densities beyond those that we observed in our surveys would be informative for assessing the utility of ratio‐ vs. more general predator‐dependent models for describing the feeding rates of generalist predators, and for assessing whether the constancy of per capita terms assumed in these models is in fact appropriate (Novak et al. ).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 2 more Smart Citations