Oxford Handbooks Online 2016
DOI: 10.1093/oxfordhb/9780199601264.013.21
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Quantification in Child Language

Abstract: This chapter addresses role of cognitive, information processing and learning mechanisms underlying children’s acquisition of quantifiers in natural language. We discuss the cognitive mechanisms that provide content to quantificational expressions, constraints on possible quantifier meanings, and the role of syntax in identifying a novel word as quantificational. We also examine the syntax and semantics of quantifiers in development, examining interactions between multiple scope bearing expressions in a single… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

0
4
0

Year Published

2020
2020
2022
2022

Publication Types

Select...
4

Relationship

0
4

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 4 publications
(4 citation statements)
references
References 76 publications
(74 reference statements)
0
4
0
Order By: Relevance
“…In more subtle cases of true or fake scope ambiguity, there are concerns that children may undershoot or overshoot what the target grammar allows. In cases of true scope ambiguity children typically show strong biases for one interpretation, generally the same interpretation that adults favor (Lidz & Musolino, 2002), though not always (Goro & Akiba, 2004;Unsworth et al, 2008), though these biases can be overcome with strong contextual support (Gualmini, 2008;Lidz, 2014;Viau et al, 2010). More worryingly, we find cases where children allow scope interpretations that are disallowed in the adult language (Goro, 2007).…”
Section: Analysis and Reanalysis In Childrenmentioning
confidence: 66%
“…In more subtle cases of true or fake scope ambiguity, there are concerns that children may undershoot or overshoot what the target grammar allows. In cases of true scope ambiguity children typically show strong biases for one interpretation, generally the same interpretation that adults favor (Lidz & Musolino, 2002), though not always (Goro & Akiba, 2004;Unsworth et al, 2008), though these biases can be overcome with strong contextual support (Gualmini, 2008;Lidz, 2014;Viau et al, 2010). More worryingly, we find cases where children allow scope interpretations that are disallowed in the adult language (Goro, 2007).…”
Section: Analysis and Reanalysis In Childrenmentioning
confidence: 66%
“…Many languages express quantification with a variety of specific (e.g., 1, 2, 3), approximate (e.g., several ), universal (e.g., every ), and comparative (e.g., more ) terms (Lidz, 2016). An understanding of this core vocabulary, as well as a command of technical terms (e.g., hypotenuse ) and specialized uses of common words (e.g., regroup, simplify, significance ), is important for representing and conveying mathematical knowledge (Schleppegrell, 2007).…”
Section: Language and Mathematical Developmentmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…While this works with ambiguous sentences such as 2, which are susceptible to a scope analysis, the non-ambiguity of the sentence in 1 renders such an analysis less informative. 2 That is, a noun phrase 2 For a review of work on the development of children's scopal interpretations, including Musolino's (1998) Observation of Isomorphism, see Lidz (2016).…”
Section: Syntactic Scope and Semantic Ontologymentioning
confidence: 99%