2005
DOI: 10.1098/rsbl.2005.0344
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Punishment and partner switching cause cooperative behaviour in a cleaning mutualism

Abstract: What are the mechanisms that prevent partners from cheating in potentially cooperative interactions between unrelated individuals? The cleaner fish Labroides dimidiatus and client reef fish both benefit from an interaction as long as the cleaner eats ectoparasites. However, the cleaner fish prefers some client mucus, which constitutes cheating. Field observations suggested that clients control such cheating by using punishment (chasing the cleaner) or by switching partners (fleeing from the cleaner). Here, we … Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

2
163
1

Year Published

2006
2006
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
4
3
1

Relationship

1
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 202 publications
(166 citation statements)
references
References 24 publications
2
163
1
Order By: Relevance
“…For example, the cleaner fish Labroides dimidiatus eats ectoparasites on its client reef fish but could also cheat by eating client tissue or mucus. Clients use three different mechanisms to enforce cooperation: (i) they avoid cleaners that have been observed cheating (partner choice); (ii) they switch to other cleaners (partner switching); and (iii) they aggressively chase uncooperative cleaners (punishment) [63,64]. After such punishment, cleaner fish act more cooperatively and are less likely to feed on mucus [63,64].…”
Section: Box 2 Controversy: the Selective Basis Of Worker Policingmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…For example, the cleaner fish Labroides dimidiatus eats ectoparasites on its client reef fish but could also cheat by eating client tissue or mucus. Clients use three different mechanisms to enforce cooperation: (i) they avoid cleaners that have been observed cheating (partner choice); (ii) they switch to other cleaners (partner switching); and (iii) they aggressively chase uncooperative cleaners (punishment) [63,64]. After such punishment, cleaner fish act more cooperatively and are less likely to feed on mucus [63,64].…”
Section: Box 2 Controversy: the Selective Basis Of Worker Policingmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Punishment is not the only way to enforce cooperation; harassing those having access to a resource [67], chasing shirkers [68] or sabotaging the attempts of cheaters [16], are different examples, and can also be found in other animals, such as mammals, fishes or insects. But humans, with their cognitive capacities for individual recognition, temporal discounting, memory, empathy and language, are uniquely gifted to develop the proximal mechanisms needed for reciprocation and in particular for punishment.…”
Section: The Limitations Of Peer-punishmentmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Therefore, they needed to swim away from the more attractive item and approach the less attractive item in order to solve the task. This subtle but perhaps important diVerence might explain why cleaners largely failed in the reverse reward contingency task, while they can easily learn to feed on a non preferred food item from a plate when a preferred item is present on the same plate in 1 cm distance (Bshary and Grutter 2005;Bshary and Grutter 2006). In these earlier experiments, each subject was allowed to continue to feed on a plate with two types of food as long as it continued to feed on the non-preferred item (Xakes).…”
Section: Methodological Considerationsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…We predicted that cleaners should be able to solve a reverse reward contingency task, based on the fact that cleaners can control impulsive behaviour in a diVerent situation, i.e. when they feed against their preference in interactions with client reef Wsh (Grutter and Bshary 2003;Bshary and Grutter 2005;Bshary et al 2007;.…”
Section: Ecological Versus Anthropocentric Approachmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation